Review published In:
Interpreting
Vol. 5:1 (2000) ► pp.6368
References
Baddeley, A. D., & Logie, R. H.
(1999) Working memory: The multiple-component model. In A. Miyake & P. Shah (Eds.), Models of working memory: Mechanisms of active maintenance and executive control. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Danks, J. H., Shreve, G. M., Fountain, S. B., & McBeath, M. K.
(Eds.) (1997) Cognitive processes in translation and interpreting. London: SAGE Publications.Google Scholar
Gambier, Y., Gile, D., & Taylor, C.
(Eds.) (1997) Conference Interpreting Current trends in research. Amsterdam: John Benjamins. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Gerver, D.
(1974a) Simultaneous listening and speaking and retention of prose. Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 261, 337–341. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
(1974b) The effects of noise on the performance of simultaneous interpreters: Accuracy of performance. Acta Psychologica, 381, 159–167. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Isham, W. P.
(1994) Memory for sentence form after simultaneous interpretation: Evidence both for and against deverbalization. In S. Lambert & B. Moser-Mercer (Eds.), Bridging the gap: Empirical research in simultaneous interpretation (pp. 191–212). Amsterdam: John Benjamins. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Isham, W. P., & Lane, H.
(1993) Simultaneous interpretation and the recall of source-language sentences. Language and Cognitive Processes, 8:3, 241–264. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Lambert, S.
(1983) Recall and recognition among conference interpreters. University of Stirling, Scotland (Unpublished doctoral dissertation).Google Scholar
Williams, S.
(1994) The application of SLA research to interpreting. Perspectives: Studies in Translatology 11, 1–28.Google Scholar