Article published In:
What Influences Influence? How the Communicative Situation Influences Persuasion
Edited by Kerstin Fischer and Jaap Ham
[Interaction Studies 22:3] 2021
► pp. 416463
References
Acerbi, A.
(2019) Cognitive attraction and online misinformation. Palgrave Communications, 5(1), 1–7. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Adadi, A., & Berrada, M.
(2018) Peeking inside the black-box: a survey on explainable artificial intelligence (XAI). IEEE access, 61, 52138–52160. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Adelmann, P. K., & Zajonc, R. B.
(1989) Facial efference and the experience of emotion. Annual Review of Psychology, 401, 249–280. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Arias, P., Soladie, C., Bouafif, O., Roebel, A., Seguier, R., & Aucouturier, J. J.
(2018) Realistic transformation of facial and vocal smiles in real-time audiovisual streams. IEEE Transactions on Affective Computing, 11(3), 507–518. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Arrieta, A. B., Díaz-Rodríguez, N., Del Ser, J., Bennetot, A., Tabik, S., Barbado, A., & Herrera, F.
(2020) Explainable Artificial Intelligence (XAI): Concepts, taxonomies, opportunities and challenges toward responsible AI. Information Fusion, 581, 82–115. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Bachorowski, J. & Owren, J.
(2001) Not all laughs are alike: Voiced but not unvoiced laughter readily elicits positive affect. Psychological Science 12.31, pp. 252–257. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Bachorowski, J. and Michael J. Owren
(2002) Vocal acoustics in emotional intelligence. Emotions and social behavior. The wisdom in feeling: Psychological processes in emotional intelligence, pp. 11–36.Google Scholar
Bachorowski, J., Smoski, J. & Owren, J.
(2001) The acoustic features of human laughter. In: The Journal of the Acoustical Society of America 110.31, pp. 1581–1597. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Bailenson, J. N.
(2006) Transformed social interaction in collaborative virtual environments. Digital media: Transformations in human communication, 255–264.Google Scholar
Bangerter, A., Mayor, E., & Knutsen, D.
(2020) Lexical entrainment without conceptual pacts? Revisiting the matching task. Journal of Memory and Language, 1141, 104–129. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Banning, M. R., & Nelson, D. L.
(1987) The effects of activity-elicited humor and group structure on group cohesion and affective responses. American Journal of Occupational Therapy, 41(8), 510–514. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Bargh, J. A., & Chartrand, T. L.
(2014) The mind in the middle: A practical guide to priming and automaticity research. In H. T. Reis & C. M. Judd (Eds.), Handbook of research methods in social and personality psychology (pp. 311–344). Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Barsade, S. G.
(2002) The ripple effect: Emotional contagion and its influence on group behavior. Administrative science quarterly, 47(4), 644–675. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Bavelas, J. B., Black, A., Lemery, C. R., & Mullett, J.
(1986) I show how you feel: Motor mimicry as a communicative act. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 501, 322–329. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Bebbington, K., MacLeod, C., Ellison, T. M., & Fay, N.
(2017) The sky is falling: Evidence of a negativity bias in the social transmission of information. Evolution and Human Behavior, 38(1), 92–101. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Bernieri, F. J., Reznick, J. S., & Rosenthal, R.
(1988) Synchrony, pseudosynchrony, and dissynchrony: Measuring the entrainment process in mother–infant interactions. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 541, 243–253. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Blakemore, S., & Frith, C.
(2005) The role of motor contagion in the prediction of action. Neuopsychologia, 43(2), 260–267. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Brody, S. & Diakopoulos, N.
(2011) Cooooooooooooooollllllllllllll‼‼‼‼‼‼‼: Using word lengthening to detect sentiment in microblogs. Proceedings of EMNLP ’11 1, 562–570.Google Scholar
Bryant, G. A.
(2020) Evolution, structure, and functions of human laughter. The handbook of communication science and biology (pp. 63–77). Routledge. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Bryant, G. A., Wang, C. S., & Fusaroli, R.
(2020) Recognizing affiliation in colaughter and cospeech. Royal Society Open Science, 7(10), 201092. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Buckley, F. H.
(2014) 14 Schadenfreude and laughter. Schadenfreude: Understanding pleasure at the misfortune of others, edited by Wilco W. van Dijk, Jaap W. Ouwerkerk, 2191. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Bush, L. K., Barr, C. L., McHugo, G. J. & Lanzetta, J.
(1989) The effects of facial control and facial mimicry on subjective reactions to comedy routines. Motivation and Emotion 131, 31–52 (1989) DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Butcher, J., & Whissell, C.
(1984) Laughter as a function of audience size, sex of the audience, and segments of the short film Duck Soup. Perceptual and Motor Skills, 59 (3), 949–950. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Cappella, J. N., & Planalp, S.
(1981) Talk and silence sequences in informal conversations: III. Interspeaker influence. Human Communication Research, 71, 117–132. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Castelvecchi, D.
(2016) Can we open the black box of AI? Nature News 538 (7623) 20. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Chapple, E. D.
(1982) Movement and sound: The musical language of body rhythms in interaction. In M. Davis (Ed.), Interaction rhythms: Periodicity in communicative behavior (pp. 31–52). New York: Human Sciences Press.Google Scholar
Chartrand, T. L., & Bargh, J. A.
(1999) The chameleon effect: The perception-behavior link and social interaction. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 761, 893–910. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Chartrand, T. L., & Jefferis, V. E.
(2003) Consequences of automatic goal pursuit and the case of nonconscious mimicry. Social judgments: Implicit and explicit processes, 290–305.Google Scholar
Chartrand, T. L., Maddux, W. W., & Lakin, J. L.
(2005) Beyond the perception-behavior link: The ubiquitous utility and motivational moderators of nonconscious mimicry. In R. R. Hassin, J. S. Uleman, & J. A. Bargh (Eds.), The new unconscious (pp. 334–361). New York: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Cheng, L. P., Marwecki, S., & Baudisch, P.
(2017, October). Mutual human actuation. Proceedings of the 30th Annual ACM Symposium on User Interface Software and Technology (pp. 797–805). DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Chivers, T.
(2019) A theory in crisis. Nature, 576(7786), 200–202. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Christopherson, L.
(2013) OMG! l2spell online: The creative vocabulary of cyberlanguage PhD thesis, University of North Carolina.
Cicourel, A. V.
(1974) Cognitive sociology: Language and meaning in social interaction.Google Scholar
Davila Ross, M., Menzler, S., Zimmermann, E.
(2007) Rapid facial mimicry in orangutan play. Biology letters 4.11, pp. 27–30.Google Scholar
Davila-Ross, M., Allcock, B., Thomas, C., & Bard, K. A.
(2011) Aping expressions? Chimpanzees produce distinct laugh types when responding to laughter of others. Emotion 11.51, p. 1013. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Davila-Ross, M., Owren, M. J., & Zimmermann, E.
(2009) Reconstructing the evolution of laughter in great apes and humans. Current Biology, 191, 1106–1111. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
DeVito, M. A., Gergle, D., & Birnholtz, J.
(2017) Algorithms ruin everything # RIPTwitter, Folk Theories, and Resistance to Algorithmic Change in Social Media. In Proceedings of the 2017 CHI conference on human factors in computing systems (pp. 3163–3174). DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Dijksterhuis, A., & Bargh, J. A.
(2001) The perception-behavior expressway: Automatic effects of social perception on social behavior. In M. Zanna (Ed.), Advances in experimental social psychology, Vol. 331 (pp. 1–40). San Diego, CA: Academic Press.Google Scholar
Dunbar, R. I.
(2012) Bridging the bonding gap: the transition from primates to humans. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences, 367(1597), 1837–1846. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Duran, N. D., Paxton, A., & Fusaroli, R.
(2019) ALIGN: Analyzing linguistic interactions with generalizable techNiques – A Python library. Psychological methods, 24(4), 419. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Eibl-Eibesfeldt, I.
(1970) Ethology: The biology of behavior. New York: Holt, Rinehart & WinstonGoogle Scholar
Eshghi, A.; Maraev, V.; Howes, C.; Hough, J.; & Mazzocconi, C.
(2019) What are you laughing at? Incremental processing of laughter in interaction. In Howes, C.; and Hough, J., editor(s), Proceedings of SemDial 2019 (LondonLogue) 2019.Google Scholar
Eslami, M., Rickman, A., Vaccaro, K., Aleyasen, A., Vuong, A., Karahalios, K., & Sandvig, C.
(2015) I always assumed that I wasn’t really that close to [her] Reasoning about Invisible Algorithms in News Feeds. Proceedings of the 33rd annual ACM conference on human factors in computing systems (pp. 153–162).Google Scholar
Firth, A.
(1996) The discursive accomplishment of normality: On ‘lingua franca’English and conversation analysis. Journal of pragmatics, 26(2), 237–259. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Fischer, K., Jensen, L. C., Zitzmann, N.
(2022) In the same boat: The Influence of Sharing the Situational Context on a Speaker’s (a Robot’s) Persuasiveness, Interaction Studies – this volume.Google Scholar
Fusaroli, R., Rączaszek-Leonardi, J., & Tylén, K.
(2014) Dialog as interpersonal synergy. New Ideas in Psychology, 32 1, 147–157. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Fusaroli, R., & Tylén, K.
(2016) Investigating conversational dynamics: Interactive alignment, Interpersonal synergy, and collective task performance. Cognitive science, 40 (1), 145–171. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Galantucci, B., & Roberts, G.
(2014) Do we notice when communication goes awry? an investigation of people’s sensitivity to coherence in spontaneous conversation. PloS one, 9(7), e103182. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Galantucci, B., Roberts, G., & Langstein, B.
(2018) Content deafness: When coherent talk just doesn’t matter. Language & Communication, 611, 29–34. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Gervais, M., & Wilson, D. S.
(2005) The evolution and functions of laughter and humor: A synthetic approach. The Quarterly Review of Biology, 801, 395–430. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Ginzburg, J., Breitholtz, E., Cooper, R., Hough, J., & Tian, Y.
(2015) Understanding laughter. Proceedings of the 20th Amsterdam Colloquium. University of Amsterdam. [URL]
Ginzburg, J., Mazzocconi, C., & Tian, Y.
(2020) Laughter as language. Glossa: A Journal of General Linguistics, 5(1), 104. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Glenn, P.
(2003) Laughter in interaction. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Glenn, P. & Holt, E.
(2013) Studies of laughter in interaction. A&C Black. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Grammer, K., & Eibl-Eibesfeldt, I.
(1990) The ritualisation of laughter. W. A. Koch (Ed.), Natürlichkeit der Sprache und der Kultur (pp. 192–214). Bochum, Germany: Brockmeyer.Google Scholar
Greatbatch, D., & Clark, T.
(2003) Displaying group cohesiveness: Humour and laughter in the public lectures of management gurus. Human relations, 56(12), 1515–1544. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Gregoromichelaki, E., Kempson, R. & Howes, C.
(2020) Actionism in syntax and semantics. In Howes, C., Dobnik, S. & Breitholtz, E. (editors), Dialogue and Perception – Extended papers from DaP2018. Gothenburg: GUPEA.Google Scholar
Guillory, J., Spiegel, J., Drislane, M., Weiss, B., Donner, W., & Hancock, J.
(2011) Upset now? Emotion contagion in distributed groups. Proceedings of the SIGCHI conference on human factors in computing systems (pp. 745–748). DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Haakana, M.
(2002) Laughter in medical interaction: From quantification to analysis, and back. Journal of Sociolinguistics 6.21: 207–235. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Hale, J., & Hamilton, A. F.
(2016) Testing the relationship between mimicry, trust and rapport in virtual reality conversations. Scientific reports, 61, 35295. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Ham, J.
(2022) Influencing Robot Influence: Personalization of Persuasive Robots, Interaction studies – in this volume.Google Scholar
Hanauska, M. and Leßmöllmann, A.
(2022) Persuasion in Science Communication: empirical findings on scientific weblogs, Interaction studies – in this volume.Google Scholar
Hancock, J. T., Gee, K., Ciaccio, K., & Lin, J. M. H.
(2008) I’m sad you’re sad: emotional contagion in CMC. Proceedings of the 2008 ACM conference on Computer supported cooperative work (pp. 295–298). DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Hancock, J. T., Naaman, M., & Levy, K.
(2020) AI-mediated communication: definition, research agenda, and ethical considerations. Journal of Computer-Mediated Communication, 25(1), 89–100. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Hatfield, E., Cacioppo, J., & Rapson, R. L.
(1994) Emotional contagion. New York, NY: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Hatfield, E., Bensman, L., Thornton, P. D., & Rapson, R. L.
(2014) New perspectives on emotional contagion: A review of classic and recent research on facial mimicry and contagion. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Healey, P. G., Purver, M., King, J., Ginzburg, J., & Mills, G. J.
(2003) Experimenting with clarification in dialogue. Proceedings of the Annual Meeting of the Cognitive Science Society (Vol. 25, No. 25).Google Scholar
Healey PGT, Purver M, Howes C
(2014) Divergence in Dialogue. PLoS ONE 9(6): e98598. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Healey, P. G., Mills, G. J., Eshghi, A., & Howes, C.
(2018) Running repairs: Coordinating meaning in dialogue. Topics in Cognitive Science, 10(2), 367–388. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Herring, S.
(1999) Interactional coherence in CMC. Journal of Computer-Mediated Communication 4 (4). DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Heyes, C.
(2011) Automatic imitation. Psychological bulletin, 137(3), 463. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Hodges, B. H.
(2014) Rethinking conformity and imitation: Divergence, convergence, and social understanding. Frontiers in psychology, 51, 726. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Hohenstein, J., & Jung, M.
(2020) AI as a moral crumple zone: The effects of AI-mediated communication on attribution and trust. Computers in Human Behavior, 1061, 106190. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
(2013) Conversation analysis and laughter. In: Chapelle, Carol A. (Ed.), The Encyclopedia of Applied Linguistics. Blackwell, Chichester, pp. 1033e1038.Google Scholar
Horton, W. S., & Gerrig, R. J.
(2002) Speakers’ experiences and audience design: Knowing when and knowing how to adjust utterances to addressees. Journal of Memory and Language, 47(4), 589–606. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Howes, C., Healey, P. G., & Purver, M.
(2010) Tracking lexical and syntactic alignment in conversation. Proceedings of the Annual Meeting of the Cognitive Science Society (Vol. 32, No. 32).Google Scholar
Jefferson, G.
(1979) A technique for inviting laughter and its subsequent acceptance declination. In G. Psathas (ed.), Everyday language studies in ethnomethodology. New York: Irvington, pp. 79–96.Google Scholar
(1984) On the organization of laughter in talk about troubles. In J. M. Atkinson & J. Heritage (eds.), Structures of social action: Studies in conversation analysis. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press, pp. 346–369.Google Scholar
Kalman, Y. M., & Gergle, D.
(2014) Letter repetitions in computer-mediated communication?: a unique link between spoken and online language. Computers in Human Behavior. 341, 187–193. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Kalman, Y. M., Scissors, L. E., Gill, A., & Gergle, D.
(2013) Online chronemics convey social information. Computers in Human Behavior 29, 3, 1260–1269. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Kadir, Z. A., Maros, M. & Hamid, B. A.
(2012) Linguistic features in the online discussion forums. International Journal of Social Science and Humanity, vol.2, no. 3, pp.276–281.Google Scholar
Kindermann, T. A., & Skinner, E. A.
(2019) Is psychology suffering from an epidemic of contagion? Moving from metaphors to theoretically derived concepts and methods in the study of social influences. Theory & Psychology, 29(6), 739–756. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
König, K.
(2019) Stance taking with ‘laugh’ particles and emojis–Sequential and functional patterns of ‘laughter’ in a corpus of German WhatsApp chats. Journal of Pragmatics, 1421, 156–170. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Koudenburg, N., Postmes, T., Gordijn, E. H., & van Mourik Broekman, A.
(2015) Uniform and complementary social interaction: distinct pathways to solidarity. PloS one, 10(6), e0129061. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Kramer, A. D., Guillory, J. E., & Hancock, J. T.
(2014) Experimental evidence of massive-scale emotional contagion through social networks. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, 111(24), 8788–8790. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
La France, M., & Broadbent, M.
(1976) Group rapport: Posture sharing as a nonverbal indicator. Group and Organization Studies, 11, 328–333. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Lelonkiewicz, J. R., Pickering, M. J., & Branigan, H. P.
(2021) Does it pay to imitate? No evidence for social gains from lexical imitation. Royal Society Open Science. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Liebman, N., & Gergle, D.
(2016, February). It’s (Not) simply a matter of time: The relationship between CMC cues and interpersonal affinity. Proceedings of the 19th ACM conference on computer-supported cooperative work & social computing (pp. 570–581). DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Martin, Rod A.
(2010) The psychology of humor: An integrative approach. Elsevier.Google Scholar
Matsumoto, D.
(1987) The role of facial response in the experience of emotion: More methodological problems and a meta-analysis. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 521, 769–774. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Mazzocconi, C.
(2019) Laughter in interaction: semantics, pragmatics, and child development. PhD thesis, Universite de Paris.
Mazzocconi, Chiara, Ye Tian & Jonathan Ginzburg
What’s your laughter doing there? A taxonomy of the pragmatic functions of laughter, in: IEEE Transactions on Affective Computing. DOI logo
McGettigan, C., Walsh, E., Jessop, R., Agnew, Z. K., Sauter, D. A., Warren, J. E., & Scott, S. K.
(2015) Individual differences in laughter perception reveal roles for mentalizing and sensorimotor systems in the evaluation of emotional authenticity. Cerebral cortex, 25(1), 246–257. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
McKay, I.
(2015) Laughing with letters: A corpus investigation of written laughter on Twitter. BA Thesis. University of Michigan.
(2020) Some distributional patterns in the use of typed laughter-derived expressions on Twitter. Journal of Pragmatics, 1661, 97–113. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
McSweeney, Michelle A.
(2016) Lol! I Didn’t Mean it: Lol as a Marker of Illocutionary Force. LSA, Austin, TX.Google Scholar
McVeigh-Schultz, J., & Isbister, K.
(2021) The Case for Weird Social in VR/XR: A Vision of Social Superpowers Beyond Meatspace. Extended Abstracts of the 2021 CHI Conference. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Mills, G.
(2011) The emergence of procedural conventions in dialogue. In Proceedings of the Annual Meeting of the Cognitive Science Society (Vol. 33, No. 33).Google Scholar
Mills, G. J.
(2014) Dialogue in joint activity: Complementarity, convergence and conventionalization. New ideas in psychology, 32 1, 158–173. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Mills, G. J., Purver, M., and Healey, P. G.
(2013) A dialogue experimentation toolkit. [URL]
Nguyen, D. T., & Fussell, S. R.
(2014) Lexical cues of interaction involvement in dyadic instant messaging conversations. Discourse Processes, 51(5–6), 468–493. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Owren, M. J., & Bachorowski, J.-A.
(2001) The evolution of emotional experience: A selfish-gene account of smiling and laughter in early hominids and humans. In T. J. Mayne & G. A. Bonanno (Eds.), Emotions: Current issues and future directions (pp. 152–191). Guilford Press.Google Scholar
Petitjean, C., Mo, E.
(2017) Hahaha: laughter as a resource to manage WhatsApp conversations. Journal of Pragmatics 1101, 1e19. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Pickering, M. J., & Garrod, S.
(2004) Toward a mechanistic psychology of dialogue. Behavioral and brain sciences, 27(2), 169–190. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Plessner, Helmuth
(1970) Laughing and crying: a study of the limits of human behavior. Northwestern University Press.Google Scholar
Provine, R. R.
(1992) Contagious laughter: Laughter is a sufficient stimulus for laughs and smiles. In: Bulletin of the Psychonomic Society 30.11, pp. 1–4. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
(1993) Laughter punctuates speech: Linguistic, social and gender contexts of laughter. In: Ethology 95.41, pp. 291–298. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
(2001) Laughter: A scientific investigation. Penguin.Google Scholar
Provine, R. R. & Fischer, K. R.
(1989) Laughing, smiling, and talking: Relation to sleeping and social context in humans. In: Ethology 83.41, pp. 295–305. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Rader, E., & Gray, R.
(2015) Understanding user beliefs about algorithmic curation in the Facebook news feed. Proceedings of the 33rd annual ACM conference on human factors in computing systems (pp. 173–182). DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Reddy, V., Williams, E., & Vaughan, A.
(2002) Sharing humour and laughter in autism and Down’s syndrome. British Journal of Psychology 93.21, pp. 219–242. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Reed, B. S.
(2020) Reconceptualizing mirroring: Sound imitation and rapport in naturally occurring interaction. Journal of Pragmatics, 1671, 131–151. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Rizzolatti, G.
(2004) The mirror-neuron system and imitation. Perspectives on Imitation: From Mirror Neurons to Memes, ed. S. Hurley, N. Chater. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.Google Scholar
(2005) The mirror neuron system and its function in humans. Anatomy and Embryology, 210(5–6), 419–421. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Rizzolatti, G., & Craighero, L.
(2004) The mirror-neuron system. Annual Review of Neuroscience, 271, 169–192. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Rizzolatti, G., Fadiga, L., Fogassi, L., & Gallese, V.
(1999) Resonance behaviors and mirror neurons. Archives Italiennes de Biologie, 1371, 85–100.Google Scholar
Roberts, G., Langstein, B., & Galantucci, B.
(2016) (In) sensitivity to incoherence in human communication. Language & Communication, 471, 15–22. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Rorschach, H.
(1927) Rorschach Test – Psychodiagnostic Plates. CaGoogle Scholar
Ross, M. D., Owren, M. J. & Zimmermann, E.
(2009) Reconstructing the evolution of laughter in great apes and humans. Current Biology 19.131, pp. 1106–1111. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
(2010) The evolution of laughter in great apes and humans. Communicative & integrative biology 3.21, pp. 191–194. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Rozin, P., & Royzman, E. B.
(2001) Negativity bias, negativity dominance, and contagion. Personality and social psychology review, 5(4), 296–320. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Ruch, W., & Ekman, P.
(2001) The expressive pattern of laughter. Emotion, qualia, and consciousness, pp. 426–443. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Scheflen, A. E.
(1964) The significance of posture in communication systems. Psychiatry, 271, 316–331. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Schegloff, E. A.
(2007) Sequence organization in interaction: A primer in conversation analysis I (Vol. 11). Cambridge university press. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Sherman, J. W. & Rivers, A. M.
(2021) There’s Nothing Social about Social Priming: Derailing the Train Wreck, Psychological Inquiry, 32:1, 1–11. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Scott, S. K., Sauter, D. & McGettigan, C.
(2010) Brain mechanisms for processing perceived emotional vocalizations in humans. Handbook of Behavioral Neuroscience. Vol. 191. Elsevier, pp. 187–197.Google Scholar
Scott, S. K., Lavan, N., Chen, S., & McGettigan, C.
(2014) The social life of laughter. Trends in cognitive sciences, 18(12), 618–620. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Scott, S. K., Young, A. W., Calder, A. J., Hellawell, D. J., Aggleton, J. P., & Johnsons, M.
(1997) Impaired auditory recognition of fear and anger following bilateral amygdala lesions. Nature, 385 (6613), 254–257. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Sidi, Y., Glikson, E., & Cheshin, A.
(2021) Do You Get What I Mean?!? The Undesirable Outcomes of (Ab) Using Paralinguistic Cues in Computer-Mediated Communication. Frontiers in psychology, 121, 1337. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Smoski, M., & Bachorowski, J. A.
(2003) Antiphonal laughter between friends and strangers. Cognition and Emotion, 171, 327–340. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Strupka, E., Niebuhr, O., & Fischer, K.
(2016) Influence of robot gender and speaker gender on prosodic entrainment in HRI. Interactive Session at the IEEE International Symposium on Robot and Human Interactive Communication (RO-MAN 2016), New York City.Google Scholar
Tian, Y., Mazzocconi, C., & Ginzburg, J.
(2016) When do we laugh?. Proceedings of the 17th Annual Meeting of the Special Interest Group on Discourse and Dialogue (pp. 360–369). DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Varnhagen, C. K., McFall, G. P., Pugh, N., Routledge, L., Sumida-MacDonald, H., & Kwong, T. E.
(2010) Lol: New language and spelling in instant messaging. Reading and writing, 23(6), 719–733. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Vinton, K. L.
(1989) Humor in the workplace: It is more than telling jokes. Small group behavior, 20 (2), 151–166. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Walther, J. B.
(2007) Selective self-presentation in computer-mediated communication: hyperpersonal dimensions of technology, language, and cognition. Computers in Human Behavior. 231, 2538–2557. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
(2016) Social information processing theory (CMC), The International Encyclopedia of Interpersonal Communication, eds C. R. Berger and M. E. Roloff (Hoboken, NJ: John Wiley & Sons, Inc), 1–13. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Warren, J. E., Sauter, D. A., Eisner, F., Wiland, J., Dresner, M. A., Wise, R. J., Rosen, S., & Scott, S. K.
(2006) Positive emotions preferentially engage an auditory–motor “mirror” system. Journal of Neuroscience, 26(50), 13067–13075. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Young, R. D., and Frye, M.
(1966) Some are laughing; some are not – why? Psycholigcal Reports 181, 747–754. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Zhang, L., & Healey, P. G.
(2018) Human, Chameleon or Nodding Dog?. Proceedings of the 20th ACM International Conference on Multimodal Interaction (pp. 428–436). DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Ziembowicz, K., & Nowak, A.
(2019) Prosody of Text Communication? How to Induce Synchronization and Coherence in Chat Conversations. International Journal of Human–Computer Interaction, 35(17), 1586–1595. DOI logoGoogle Scholar