Article published in:
Interaction Studies
Vol. 16:1 (2015) ► pp. 5467
References

References

Appel, M., & Richter, T.
(2007) Persuasive effects of fictional narratives increase over time. Media Psychology, 10, 113–134.Google Scholar
Aron, A., Aron, E.N., Tudor, M., & Nelson, G.
(1991) Close relationships as including other in the self. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 60, 241–253. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Brass, M., Bekkering, H. & Prinz, W.
(2001) Movement observation affects movement execution in a simple response task. Acta Psychologica, 106, 3–22. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Böckler, A., Knoblich, G., & Sebanz, N.
(2012) Effects of a coactor’s focus of attention on task performance. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 38, 1404–1415. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Böckler, A., & Zwickel, J.
(2013) Influences of spontaneous perspective taking on spatial and identity processing of faces. Social, Cognitive, and Affective Neuroscience, 8, 735–740. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Burgoon, J.K., Bonito, J.A., Bengtsson, B., Cederberg, C., Lundeberg, M., & Allspach, L.
(2000) Interactivity in human-computer interaction: A study of credibility, understanding, and influence. Computers in Human Behavior, 16, 553–574. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Castelli, F., Happé, F., Frith, U., & Frith, C.
(2000) Movement and mind: A functional imaging study of perception and interpretation of complex intentional movement patterns. Neuroimage, 12, 314–325. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Colzato, L.S., de Bruijn, E., & Hommel, B.
(2012a) Up to “me” or up to “us”? The impact of self-construal priming on cognitive self-other integration. Frontiers in Psychology, 3, 341. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Colzato, L.S., Zech, H., Hommel, B., Verdonschot, R., van den Wildenberg, W., & Hsieh, S.
(2012b) Lovingkindness brings lovingkindness: The impact of Buddhism on cognitive self-other integration. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 19, 541–545. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Cross, E.S., Liepelt, R., de C Hamilton, A.F., Parkinson, J., Ramsey, R., Stadler, W., & Prinz, W.
(2012) Robotic movement preferentially engages the action observation network. Human Brain Mapping, 9, 2238–2254. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Dijksterhuis, A., & Bargh, J.A.
(2001) The perception-behavior expressway: Automatic effects of social perception on social behavior. Advances in Experimental Social Psychology, 33, 1–40. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Dittrich, K., Rothe, A., & Klauer, K.C.
(2012) Increased spatial salience in the social Simon task: A response-coding account of spatial compatibility effects. Attention, Perception, & Psychophysics, 74, 911–929. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Dolk, T., Hommel, B., Colzato, L.S., Schütz-Bosback, S., Prinz, W., & Liepelt, R.
(2011) How “social“ is the social Simon effect? Frontiers in Psychology, 2, 84. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Dolk, T., Hommel, B., Prinz, W., & Liepelt, R.
(2013) The (not so) Social Simon effect: A referential coding account. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 39, 1248–1260. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Dolk, T., Liepelt, R., Prinz, W., & Fiehler, K.
(2013) Visual experience determines the use of external reference frames in joint action control. PLoS One, 8, e59008. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Fukuda, H., & Ueda, K.
(2010) Interaction with a moving object affects one’s perception of its animacy. International Journal of Social Robotics, 2, 187–193. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Epley, N., Waytz, A., & Cacioppo, J.T.
(2007) On seeing human: A three-factor theory of anthropomorphism. Psychological Review, 114, 864–886. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Gazzola, V., Rizzolatti, G., Wicker, B., & Keysers, C.
(2007) The anthropomorphic brain: The mirror neuron system responds to human and robotic actions. NeuroImage, 35, 1674–1684. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Green, M.C., & Brock, T.C.
(2000) The role of transportation in the persuasiveness of public narratives. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 29, 701–721. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Green, M.C., Brock, T.C., & Kaufman, G.F.
(2004) Understanding media enjoyment: The role of transportation into narrative worlds. Communication Theory, 14, 311–327. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Guagnano, D., Rusconi, E., & Umiltà, C.A.
(2010) Sharing a task or sharing a space? On the effect of the confederate in action coding in a detection task. Cognition, 114, 348–355. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Hard, B.M., Tversky, B., & Lang, D.S.
(2006) Making sense of abstract events: Building event schemas. Memory & Cognition, 14, 1221–1253. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Hommel, B., Colzato, L.S., & van den Wildenberg, W.P.M.
(2009) How social are task representations? Psychological Science, 7, 794–798. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Kilner, J.M., Paulignan, Y., & Blakemore, S.J.
(2003) An interference effect of observed biological movement on action. Current Biology, 13, 522–525. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Liepelt, R., & Brass, M.
(2010) Top-down modulation of motor priming by belief about animacy. Experimental Psychology, 57, 221–227. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Liepelt, R., Schneider, J.C., Aichert, D., Wöstmann, N., Dehning, S., Möller, H.J., Riedel, M., Dolk, T., & Ettinger, U.
(2012) Action blind: Disturbed self-other integration in Schizophrenia. Neuropsychologia, 50, 3775–3780. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Liepelt, R., Wenke, D., & Fischer, R.
(2013) Effects of feature integration in a hands-crossed version of the social simon paradigm. Psychological Research, 77, 240–248. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Müller, B.C.N., Brass, M., Kühn, S., Tsai, C.-C., Nieuwboer, W., Dijksterhuis, A., & van Baaren, R.B.
(2011a) When Pinocchio acts like a human, a wooden hand becomes embodied. Action co-representation for non-biological agents. Neuropsychologia, 49, 1373–1377. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Müller, B.C.N., Kühn, S., van Baaren, R.B., Dotsch, R., Brass, M., & Dijksterhuis, A.
(2011b) Perspective taking eliminates differences in co-representation of out-group members’ actions. Experimental Brain Research, 211, 423–428. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Müller, B.C.N., van Baaren, R.B., van Someren, D.H., & Dijksterhuis, A.
(2014) A present for Pinocchio: On when non-biological agents become real. Social Cognition, 32, 382–396. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Press, C.
(2011) Action observation and robotic agents: Learning and anthropomorphism. Neuroscience and Biobehavioral Reviews, 35, 1410–1418. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Prinz, W.
(1997) Perception and action planning. European Journal of Cognitive Psychology, 9, 129–154. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Ramnani, N., & Miall, R.C.
(2004) A system in the human brain for predicting the actions of others. Nature Neuroscience, 7, 85–90. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Ramsey, R., & Hamilton de, C.A.F.
(2010) Triangles have goals too: Understanding action representation in lef aIPS. Neuropsychologia, 48, 2773–2776. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Samson, D., Apperly, I.A., Braithwaite, J.J., Andrews, B.J., & Bodley Scott, S.E.
(2010) Seeing it their way: Evidence for rapid and involuntary computation of what other people see. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 36, 1255–1266. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Schneider, E.F., Lang, A., Shin, M., & Bradley, S.D.
(2004) Death with a story: How story impacts emotional, motivational, and physiological responses to first-person shooter video games. Human Communication Research, 30, 361–375. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Schultz, J., Friston, K.J., O‘Doherty, J., Wolpert, D.M., & Frith, C.D.
(2005) Activation in posterior superior temporal sulcus parallels parameter inducing the percept of animacy. Neuron, 45, 625–635. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Sebanz, N., Bekkering, H., & Knoblich, G.
(2006) Joint action: Bodies and minds moving together. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 10, 70–76. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Sebanz, N., Knoblich, G., & Prinz, W.
(2003) Representing others’ actions: Just like one’s own? Cognition, 8, 11–21. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Stezel, A., Chinellato, E., del Pobil, Á.P., Lappe, M., & Liepelt, R.
(2013) How deeply do we include robotic agents in the self? International Journal of Humanoid robotics, 10, 1–13. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Stenzel, A., Chinellato, E., Tirado Bou, M.A., del Pobil, Á.P., Lappe, M., & Liepelt, R.
(2012) When humanoid robots become human-like interaction partners: Co-representation of robotic actions. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 38, 1073–1077. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Stosic, M., Brass, M., van Hoeck, N., Ma, N., & van Overwalle, F.
(2014) Understanding simple goal-directed actions of shapes: The role of agency in mirror system activation. NeuroImage, 86, 264–269. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Tai, Y.F., Scherfler, C., Brooks, D.J., Sawamoto, N., & Castiello, U.
(2004) The human premotor cortex is ‘mirror’ only for biological actions. Current Biology, 14, 117–120. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Teufel, C., Fletcher, P.C., & Davis, G. (2010) Seeing other minds: Attributed mental states influence perception. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 14, 376–382. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Tsai, C.-C., & Brass, M.
(2007) Does the human motor system simulates Pinocchio’s actions? Coaction with a human hand versus a wooden hand in a dyadic interaction. Psychological Science, 18, 1058–1062. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Tsai, C.-C., Kuo, W.-J., Hung, D.L., & Tzeng, O.J.L.
(2008) Action co-representation is tuned to other humans. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 20, 2015–2024. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Tsai, C.-C., Kuo, W.-J., Jing, J.-T., Hung, D.L., & Tzeng, O.J.L.
(2006) A common coding framework in self-other interaction: Evidence from joint action task. Experimental Brain Research, 175, 353–362. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Tversky, B., & Hard, B.M.
(2009) Embodied and disembodied cognition: Spatial perspective-taking. Cognition, 110, 124–129. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Wheatley, T., Milleville, S.C., & Martin, A.
(2007) Understanding animate agents: Distinct roles for the social network and mirror system. Psychological Science, 18, 469–474. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Wilson, M., & Knoblich, G.
(2005) The case of motor involvement in perceiving conspecifics. Psychological Bulletin, 131, 460–473. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Zwickel, J.
(2009) Agency attribution and visuo-spatial perspective taking. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 16, 1089–1093. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Cited by

Cited by 4 other publications

Müller, Barbara C. N., Shengnan Chen, Sari R. R. Nijssen, Simone Kühn & Thomas A Stoffregen
2018. How (not) to increase older adults’ tendency to anthropomorphise in serious games. PLOS ONE 13:7  pp. e0199948 ff. Crossref logo
Müller, Barbara C. N., Xin Gao, Sari R. R. Nijssen & Tom G. E. Damen
2021. I, Robot: How Human Appearance and Mind Attribution Relate to the Perceived Danger of Robots. International Journal of Social Robotics 13:4  pp. 691 ff. Crossref logo
Nijssen, Sari R.R., Carolina Pletti, Markus Paulus & Barbara C.N. Müller
2021. Does agency matter? Neural processing of robotic movements in 4- and 8-year olds. Neuropsychologia 157  pp. 107853 ff. Crossref logo
van der Weiden, Anouk, Henk Aarts, Merel Prikken & Neeltje E. M. van Haren
2016. Individual differences in action co-representation: not personal distress or subclinical psychotic experiences but sex composition modulates joint action performance. Experimental Brain Research 234:2  pp. 499 ff. Crossref logo

This list is based on CrossRef data as of 14 september 2021. Please note that it may not be complete. Sources presented here have been supplied by the respective publishers. Any errors therein should be reported to them.