Article published In:
Coordination, Collaboration and Cooperation: Interdisciplinary perspectives
Edited by Federica Amici and Lucas M. Bietti
[Interaction Studies 16:3] 2015
► pp. 341360
References
Alexander, R.D
(1987) The biology of moral systems. New York: Walter de Gruyter.Google Scholar
Barcaly, P
(2011) The evolution of charitable behaviour and the power of reputation. In S.C. Roberts (Ed.), Applied evolutionary psychology. Oxford: Oxford University Press. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Amici, F., Aureli, F., Visalberghi, E., & Call, J
(2009) Spider monkeys (Ateles geoffroyi) and capuchin monkeys (Cebus apella) follow gaze around barriers: Evidence for perspective taking? Journal of Comparative Psychology, 123(4), 368–374. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Amici, F., Visalberghi, E., & Call, J
(2014) Lack of prosociality in great apes, capuchin monkeys and spider monkeys: Convergent evidence from two different food distribution tasks. Proceedings of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences , 281(1699).
Boesch, C
(1994) Cooperative hunting in wild chimpanzees. Animal Behaviour, 48(3), 653–667. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Bonanni, R., & Cafazzo, S
(2014) The social organization of a population of free-ranging dogs in a suburban area of Rome: A reassessment of the effects of domestication on dog behaviour. In J. Kaminski & S. Marshall-Pescini (Eds.), The social dog: Behaviour and cognition (pp. 65–104). San Diego, London, Waltham: Elsevier publishers. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Bräuer, J
(2014) What dogs understand about humans. In J. Kaminski & S. Marshall-Pescini (Eds.), The social dog: Behaviour and cognition (pp. 295–317). San Diego, London, Waltham: Elsevier publishers. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Bräuer, J., Bös, M., Call, J., & Tomasello, M
(2013) Domestic dogs (Canis familiaris) coordinate their actions in a problem-solving task. Animal Cognition, 16(2), 273–285. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Bräuer, J., Call, J., & Tomasello, M
(2005) All great ape species follow gaze to distant locations and around barriers. Journal of Comparative Psychology, 119(2), 145–154. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
(2006) Are apes really inequity averse? Proceedings of Royal Society London, 2731, 3123–3128. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
(2007) Chimpanzees really know what others can see in a competitive situation. Animal Cognition, 10(4), 439–448. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Bräuer, J., Call, J., & Tomasello, M (2009) Are apes inequity averse? New data on the token-exchange paradigm. American Journal of Primatology, 71(2), 175–181. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Bräuer, J., & Hanus, D
(2012) Fairness in non-human primates? Social Justice Research, 25(3), 256–276. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Bräuer, J., Schönefeld, K., & Call, J
(2013) When do dogs help humans? Applied Animal Behaviour Science, 148(1-2), 138–149. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Brosnan, S.F., & de Waal, F
(2014) Evolution of responses to (un)fairness. Science, 346(6207). DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Brosnan, S.F., Hopper, L.M., Richey, S., Freeman, H.D., Talbot, C.F., Gosling, S.D., Lambeth, S.P., & Schapiro, S.J
(2015) Personality influences responses to inequity and contrast in chimpanzees. Animal Behaviour, 1011, 75–87. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Brosnan, S.F., Schiff, H.C., & Waal, F.B.M. d
(2005) Tolerance for inequity may increase with social closeness in chimpanzees. Proceedings B, 2721, 253–258. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Bshary, R., & Bergmüller, R
(2008) Distinguishing four fundamental approaches to the evolution of helping. Journal of Evolutionary Biology, 211, 405–420. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Bullinger, A., Zimmerman, F., Kaminski, J., & Tomasello, M
(2011) Different social motives in the gestural communication of chimpanzees and human children. Developmental Science, 14(1), 58–68. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Burkart, J.M., Fehr, E., Efferson, C., & van Schaik, C.P
(2007) Other-regarding preferences in a non-human primate: Common marmosets provision food altruistically. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America , 104(50), 19762–19766. DOI logo
Burkart, J.M., Hrdy, S.B., & Van Schaik, C.P
(2009) Cooperative breeding and human cognitive evolution. Evolutionary Anthropology: Issues, News, and Reviews, 18(5), 175–186. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Buttelmann, D., Carpenter, M., Call, J., & Tomasello, M
(2007) Enculturated chimpanzees imitate rationally. Developmental Science, 10(4), F31–F38. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Call, J., Hare, B., Carpenter, M., & Tomasello, M
(2004) 'unwilling’ versus 'unable’: Chimpanzees' understanding of human intentional action. Developmental Science, 7(4), 488–498. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Call, J., Hare, B., & Tomasello, M
(2001) Gaze following in five non-human primate Species. Folia Primatologica, 72(6), 347–348. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Call, J., & Tomasello, M
(2008) Does the chimpanzee have a theory of mind? 30 years later. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 12(5), 187–192. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Chaumette, M., Dale, R., Dania, R., Marshall-Pescini, S., & Range, F
(2014) Pro-social behavior in pet dogs. Paper presented at the 4th Canine Science Forum, Lincoln. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Clavien, C
(2012) Altruistic emotional motivation: An argument in favour of psychological altruism. In K. Plaisance & T. Reydon (Eds.), Philosophy of behavioural biology Vol. 2821, (pp. 275–296). London, UK: Springer. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Connor, R.C
(1996) Partner preferences in by-product mutualisms and the case of predator inspection in fish. Animal Behaviour, 511, 451–454. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Coppinger, R., & Coppinger, L
(2001) Dogs: A startling new understanding of canine origin, behaviour, and evolution. New York, NY, US: Scribner. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Dale, R., Chaumette, M., Dania, R., Marshall-Pescini, S., & Range, F
(2014) Investigating prosocial tendencies in pet dogs. Paper presented at the 4th Canine Science Forum, Lincoln. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Dalziel, D.J., Uthman, B.M., McGorray, S.P., & Reep, R.L (2003) Seizure-alert dogs: A review and preliminary study. Seizure-European Journal of Epilepsy, 12(2), 115–120. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Emlen, S.T., & Wrege, P.H
(1988) The role of kinship in helping decisions among whitefronted bee-eaters. Behavioural Ecology and Sociobiolgy, 23(5), 305–315. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Fehr, E., & Fischbacher, U
(2003) The nature of human altruism. Nature, 425(6960), 785–791. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Hadjichrysanthou, C., & Broom, M
(2012) When should animals share food? Game theory applied to kleptoparasitic populations with food sharing. Behavioral Ecology, 23(5), 977–991. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Hamann, K., Warneken, F., Greenberg, J., & Tomasello, M
(2011) Collaboration encourages equal sharing in children but not chimpanzees. Nature, 4761, 328–331. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Hamilton, W.D
(1964) The genetical evolution of social behaviour. I & II. Journal of Theoretical Biology, 7(1), 1–52. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Hare, B., Call, J., Agnetta, B., & Tomasello, M
(2000) Chimpanzees know what conspecifics do and do not see. Animal Behaviour, 59(4), 771–785. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Hare, B., & Tomasello, M
(1999) Domestic dogs (Canis familiaris) use human and conspecific social cues to locate hidden food. Journal of Comparative Psychology, 113(2), 173–177. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
(2004) Chimpanzees are more skilful in competitive than in cooperative cognitive tasks. Animal Behaviour, 68(3), 571–581. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
(2005) Human-like social skills in dogs? Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 9(9), 439–444. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Hay, D.F., Caplan, M., Castle, J., & Stimson, C.A
(1991) Does sharing become increasingly "rational" in the second year of life? Developmental Psychology, 27(6), 987–993. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Horner, V., Carter, J.D., Suchak, M., & de Waal, F.B.M
(2011) Spontaneous prosocial choice by chimpanzees. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America, 108(33), 13847–13851. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Jensen, K
(2012a) Social regard: Evolving a psychology of cooperation. In J. Mitani, J. Call, P. Kappeler, R. Palombit & J. Silk (Eds.), The evolution of primate societies (pp. 565–584). Chicago, USA: Chicago University Press. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
(2012b) Other-regarding sentiments - decisions with feeling. In P. Hammerstein & J. Stevens (Eds.), Evolving the mechanisms of decision making: Toward a darwinian decision theory (pp. 299–318). Cambridge: MIT Press.Google Scholar
Jensen, K., Hare, B., Call, J., & Tomasello, M
(2006) What´s in it for me? Self-regard precludes altruism and spite in chimpanzees. Proceedings of Royal Society London, Series B Biological Sciences, 273(1589), 1013–1021. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Jensen, K., Vaish, A., & Schmidt, M
(2014) The emergence of human prosociality: Aligning with others through feelings, concerns, and norms. Frontiers in Psychology, 5(822). DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Kaminski, J., Riedel, J., Call, J., & Tomasello, M
(2005) Domestic goats, capra hircus, follow gaze direction and use social cues in an object choice task. Animal Behaviour, 69(1), 11–18. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Kaminski, J., & Marshall-Pescini, S
(2014) The social dog: Behaviour and cognition. Elsevier publishers. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Kaminski, J., Neumann, M., Bräuer, J., Call, J., & Tomasello, M (2011) Domestic dogs communicate to request and not to inform. Animal Behaviour, 82(4), 651–658. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Kaminski, J., Nitzschner, M., Wobber, V., Tennie, C., Bräuer, J., Call, J., & Tomasello, M
(2011) Do dogs distinguish rational from irrational acts? Animal Behaviour, 81(1), 195–203. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Kaminski, J., & Nitzschner, M
(2013) Do dogs get the point?: A review of dog–human communication ability. Learning and Motivation, 44(4), 294–302. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Kobayashi, H., & Kohshima, S
(2001) Evolution of the human eye as a device for communication In Matsuzawa, Tetsuro (Ed). Primate research institute; Kyoto university. (2001). Primate origins of human cognition and behaviour. (pp. 383–401). New York, NY, US: Springer-Verlag Publishing. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Lehmann, L., & Keller, L
(2006) The evolution of cooperation and altruism – a general framework and a classification of models. Journal of Evolutionary Biology, 191, 1365–1376. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Lim, K., Fisher, M., & Burns-Cox, C.J
(1992) Type 1 Diabetics and their Pets. Diabetic Medicine, 91, S3–S4. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Liszkowski, U., Carpenter, M., Striano, T., & Tomasello, M
(2006) 12- and 18-month-olds point to provide information for others. [Original]. Journal of Cognition and Development, 7(2), 173–187. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Liszkowski, U., Carpenter, M., & Tomasello, M
(2008) Twelve-month-olds communicate helpfully and appropriately for knowledgeable and ignorant partners. Cognition, 108(3), 732–739. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Macpherson, K., & Roberts, W.A
(2006) Do dogs (canis familiaris) seek help in an emergency? Journal of Comparative Psychology, 120(2), 113–119. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Marshall-Pescini, S., Ceretta, M., & Prato-Previde, E
(2014) Do domestic dogs understand human actions as goal-directed? PLOS ONE, 9(9), e106530. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Mcgowan, A., Hatchwell, B.J., & Woodburn, R.J.W
(2003) The effect of helping behaviour on the survival of juvenile and adult long-tailed tits aegithalos caudatus. Journal of Animal Ecology, 72(3), 41–99. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Melis, A.P., Hare, B., & Tomasello, M
(2008) Do chimpanzees reciprocate received favours? Animal Behaviour, 76(3), 951–962. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Melis, A.P., Schneider, A., & Tomasello, M
(2011) Chimpanzees share food in the same way after collaborative and individual food acquisition. Animal Behaviour, 82(3), 485–493. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Melis, A.P., & Semmann, D
(2010) How is human cooperation different? Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society of London B Biological Sciences, 365(1553), 2663–2674. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Melis, A.P., & Tomasello, M
(2013) Chimpanzees’ (Pan troglodytes) strategic helping in a collaborative task. Biology Letters, 9(2), 20130009. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Melis, A.P., Warneken, F., Jensen, K., Schneider, A.-C., Call, J., & Tomasello, M
(2011) Chimpanzees help conspecifics obtain food and non-food items. Proceedings of the Royal Society of London B Biological Sciences, 278(1710), 1405–1413. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Miklósi, A
(2007) Dog behaviour, evolution, and cognition. Oxford: Oxford University Press. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Miklósi, A., Polgárdi, R., Topál, J., & Csányi, V
(2000) Intentional behaviour in dog-human communication: An experimental analysis of "showing' behaviour in the dog. Animal Cognition, 3(3), 159–166. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Naderi, S., Miklósi, Á., Doka, A., & Csányi, V (2001) Cooperative interactions between blind persons and their dogs. Applied Animal Behaviour Science, 74(1), 59–80. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Petter, M., Musolino, E., Roberts, W.A., & Cole, M
(2009) Can dogs (canis familiaris) detect human deception? Behavioural Processes, 82(2), 109–118. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Phillips, W., Barnes, J., Mahajan, N., Yamaguchi, M., & Santos, L
(2009) 'Unwilling' versus 'unable': Capuchin monkeys' (Cebus apella) understanding of human intentional action. Developmental Science, 12(6), 938–945. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Price, E.O
(1984) Behavioral aspects of animal domestication. Quarterly Review of Biology, 59(1), 1–32. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Prinz, J
(2007) The emotional construction of morals. Oxford: Oxford University Press. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Range, F., Virányi, Z., & Huber, L
(2007) Selective imitation in domestic dogs. Current Biology, 17(10), 868–872. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Range, F., Horn, L., Virányi, Z., & Huber, L
(2009) The absence of reward induces inequity aversion in dogs. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America, 106(1), 340–345. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Ruusila, V., & Pesonen, M
(2004) Interspecific cooperation in human (Homo sapiens) hunting: The benefits of a barking dog (Canis familiaris). Annual Zoological Fennici, 411, 545–549. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Savalli, C., Ades, C., & Gaunet, F
(2014) Are dogs able to communicate with their owners about a desirable food in a referential and intentional way? PLOS ONE, 9(9), e108003. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Schloegl, C., Kotrschal, K., & Bugnyar, T
(2008) Do common ravens (corvus corax) rely on human or conspecific gaze cues to detect hidden food? Animal Cognition, 11(2), 231–241. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Serpell, J.E
(1995) The domestic dog: Its evolution, behaviour and interactions with people. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Silk, J., Brosnan, S., Henrich, J., Lambeth, S., & Shapiro, S.J
(2013) Chimpanzees share food for many reasons: The role of kinship, reciprocity, social bonds and harassment on food transfers. Animal Behaviour, 85(5), 941–947. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Silk, J.B., Brosnan, S.F., Vonk, J., Henrich, J., Povinelli, D.J., Richardson, A.S., Lambeth, S.P., Mascaro, J. & Schapiro, S.J
(2005) Chimpanzees are indifferent t the welfare of unrelated group members. Nature, 4371, 1357–1359. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Stevens, J.R
(2004) The selfish nature of generosity: Harassment and food sharing in primates. Proceedings of the Royal Society of London B Biological Sciences, 271(1538), 451–456. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Stevens, J.R., & Gilby, I.C
(2004) A conceptual framework for nonkin food sharing: Timing and currency of benefits. Animal Behaviour, 67(4), 603–614. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Stevens, J.R., & Hauser, M.D
(2004) Why be nice? psychological constraints on the evolution of cooperation. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 8(2), 60–65. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Svartberg, K., & Forkman, B
(2002) Personality traits in the domestic dog (Canis familiaris). Applied Animal Behaviour Science, 79(2), 133–156. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Thornton, A., & McAuliffe, K
(2006) Teaching in wild meerkats. Science, 313(5784), 227–229. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Tomasello, M., Hare, B., Lehmann, H., & Call, J (2007) Reliance on head versus eyes in the gaze following of great apes and human infants: The cooperative eye hypothesis. Journal of Human Evolution, 52(3), 314–320. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Tomasello, M., Melis, A.P., Tennie, C., Wyman, E., & Herrmann, E
(2012) Two key steps in the evolution of cooperation: The interdependence hypothesis. Current Anthropology, 53(6), 673–692. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Topál, J
(2014) Dogs` sensitivity to human ostensive cues - a unique adaptation? In J. Kaminski & S. Marshall-Pescini (Eds.), The social dog: Behaviour and cognition (pp. 319–346). San Diego, London, Waltham: Elsevier publishers. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Trivers, R.L
(1971) The evolution of reciprocal altruism. Quarterly Review of Biology, 46(1), 35–57. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Ueno, A., & Matsuzawa, T
(2004) Food transfer between chimpanzee mothers and their infants. Primates, 451, 231–239. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Virányi, Z., Topál, J., Miklósi, Á., & Csányi, V
(2006) A nonverbal test of knowledge attribution: a comparative study on dogs and children. Animal Cognition, 9(1), 13–26. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
von Frisch, K
(1971) Bees: Their vision, chemical senses, and language. Ithaca: Cornell University Press. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Warneken, F., Chen, F., & Tomasello, M
(2006) Cooperative activities in young children and chimpanzees. Child Development, 77(3), 640–663. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Warneken, F., Hare, B., Melis, A.P., Hanus, D., & Tomasello, M
(2007) Spontaneous altruism by chimpanzees and young children. PLoS Biology, 5(7), e184. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Warneken, F., Lohse, K., Melis, A.P., & Tomasello, M
(2011) Young children share the spoils after collaboration. Psychological Science, 22(2), 267–273. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Warneken, F., & Tomasello, M
(2006) Altruistic helping in human infants and young chimpanzees. Science, 311(5765), 1301–1303. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
(2007) Helping and cooperation at 14 months of age. Infancy, 11(3), 271–294. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
(2008) Extrinsic rewards undermine altruistic tendencies in 20-month-olds. Developmental Psychology, 44(6), 1785–1788. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
(2009a) The roots of human altruism. British Journal of Psychology, 100(3), 455–471. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
(2009b) Varieties of altruism in children and chimpanzees. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 13(9), 397–402. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Wilkinson, A., Mandl, I., Bugnyar, T., & Huber, L
(2010) Gaze following in the red-footed tortoise (Geochelone carbonaria). Animal Cognition, 13(5), 765–769. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Wispé, L.G
(1972) Positive forms of social behavior: An overview. Journal of Social Issues, 28(3), 1–19. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Yamamoto, S., Humle, T., & Tanaka, M (2012) Chimpanzees’ flexible targeted helping based on an understanding of conspecifics’ goals. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, 109(9), 3588–3592. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
(2009b) Selfish strategies develop in social problem situations in chimpanzee (Pan troglodytes) mother–infant pairs. Animal Cognition, 121, 27–36. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Zimmermann, F., Zemke, F., Call, J., & Gómez, J.C
(2009) Orangutans (Pongo pygmaeus) and bonobos (Pan paniscus) point to inform a human about the location of a tool. Animal Cognition, 12(2), 347–358. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Cited by

Cited by 12 other publications

Bender, Yana, Juliane Bräuer & Stefan R. Schweinberger
2023. What makes a good dog-owner team? – A systematic review about compatibility in personality and attachment. Applied Animal Behaviour Science 260  pp. 105857 ff. DOI logo
Bender, Yana, Tim Matschkowski, Stefan R. Schweinberger & Juliane Bräuer
2023. “An Aid with Soul”—Understanding the Determinants of Guide Dog-Owner Compatibility from Qualitative Interviews. Animals 13:17  pp. 2751 ff. DOI logo
Bhattacharjee, Debottam & Anindita Bhadra
2020. Humans Dominate the Social Interaction Networks of Urban Free-Ranging Dogs in India. Frontiers in Psychology 11 DOI logo
Bräuer, Juliane & Yana Bender
2023. Warum die Vergleichende Psychologie auf den Hund gekommen ist. Zeitschrift für Entwicklungspsychologie und Pädagogische Psychologie 55:1  pp. 3 ff. DOI logo
Bräuer, Juliane, Daniel Hanus, Simone Pika, Russell Gray & Natalie Uomini
2020. Old and New Approaches to Animal Cognition: There Is Not “One Cognition”. Journal of Intelligence 8:3  pp. 28 ff. DOI logo
Chira, Angela M., Kathryn Kirby, Theresa Epperlein & Juliane Bräuer
2023. Function predicts how people treat their dogs in a global sample. Scientific Reports 13:1 DOI logo
Dale, Rachel, Sylvain Palma-Jacinto, Sarah Marshall-Pescini, Friederike Range & Elsa Addessi
2019. Wolves, but not dogs, are prosocial in a touch screen task. PLOS ONE 14:5  pp. e0215444 ff. DOI logo
Donnier, Sasha, Gyula Kovács, Linda S. Oña, Juliane Bräuer & Federica Amici
2020. Experience has a limited effect on humans’ ability to predict the outcome of social interactions in children, dogs and macaques. Scientific Reports 10:1 DOI logo
Leete, Jessica Ann & Jennifer Vonk
2022. In mixed company: two macaws are self-regarding in a symbolic prosocial choice task. Behavioral Ecology and Sociobiology 76:1 DOI logo
Samet, Lauren E., Helen Vaterlaws-Whiteside, Naomi D. Harvey, Melissa M. Upjohn & Rachel A. Casey
2022. Exploring and Developing the Questions Used to Measure the Human–Dog Bond: New and Existing Themes. Animals 12:7  pp. 805 ff. DOI logo
Schünemann, Britta, Judith Keller, Hannes Rakoczy, Tanya Behne & Juliane Bräuer
2021. Dogs distinguish human intentional and unintentional action. Scientific Reports 11:1 DOI logo
Van Bourg, Joshua, Jordan Elizabeth Patterson, Clive D. L. Wynne & Valerio Capraro
2020. Pet dogs (Canis lupus familiaris) release their trapped and distressed owners: Individual variation and evidence of emotional contagion. PLOS ONE 15:4  pp. e0231742 ff. DOI logo

This list is based on CrossRef data as of 31 march 2024. Please note that it may not be complete. Sources presented here have been supplied by the respective publishers. Any errors therein should be reported to them.