Article published in:
Interaction Studies
Vol. 17:2 (2016) ► pp. 155179
References
Berger, C. R.
(2000) Goal detection and efficiency: Neglected aspects of message production. Communication Theory, 101, 156–166. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Berger, C. R., & Kellermann, K.
(1994) Acquiring social information. In J. A. Daly & J. M. Wiemann (Eds.), Strategic interpersonal communication (pp. 1–31). Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum.Google Scholar
Bogdan, R. J.
(1997) Interpreting minds: The evolution of a practice. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Bower, G. H., Black, J. B., & Turner, T. J.
(1979) Scripts in memory for text. Cognitive Psychology, 111, 177–220. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Chun, W. Y., Spiegel, S., & Kruglanski, A. W.
(2002) Assimilative behavior identification can also be resource dependent: The unimodel perspective on personal-attribution phases. Journal of Personality & Social Psychology, 831, 542–555. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
DeCarlo, T. E.
(2005) The effects of sales message and suspicion of ulterior motives on salesperson evaluation. Journal of Consumer Psychology, 151, 238–249. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Dijksterhuis, A., Chartrand, T. L., & Aarts, H.
(2007) Effects of priming and perception on social behavior and goal pursuit. In J. A. Bargh (Ed.), Social psychology and the unconscious: The automaticity of higher mental processes (pp. 51–131). New York: Psychological Press.Google Scholar
Dik, G., & Aarts, H.
(2007) Behavioral cues to others’ motivation and goal pursuits: The perception of effort facilitates goal inference and contagion. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 431, 727–737. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Dudley, M. G., & Harris, M. J.
(2003) To think or not to think: The moderating role of need for cognition in expectancy-consistent impression formation. Personality and Individual Differences, 351, 1657–1667. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Eddington, K. M., Dolcos, F., Cabeza, R., Krishnan, K. R. K., & Strauman, T. J.
(2007) Neural correlates of promotion and prevention goal activation: An fMRI study using an idiographic approach. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 191, 1152–1162. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Fein, S.
(1996) Effects of suspicion on attributional thinking and the correspondence bias. Journal of Personality & Social Psychology, 701, 1164–1184. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Fein, S., & Hilton, J. L.
(1994) Judging others in the shadow of suspicion. Motivation & Emotion, 181, 167–198. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Fein, S., Hilton, J. L., & Miller, D. T.
(1990) Suspicion of ulterior motivation and the correspondence bias. Journal of Personality & Social Psychology, 581, 753–764. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Fitzsimons, G. M., & Bargh, J. A.
(2003) Thinking of you: Nonconscious pursuit of interpersonal goals associated with relationship partners. Journal of Personality & Social Psychology, 841, 148–163. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Forgas, J. P., & East, R.
(2008) On being happy and gullible: Mood effects on skepticism and the detection of deception. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 441, 1362–1367. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Francik, E. P., & Clark, H. H.
(1985) How to make requests that overcome obstacles to compliance. Journal of Memory and Language, 241, 560–584. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Gilbert, D. T., & Hixon, J. G.
(1991) The trouble of thinking: Activation and application of stereotypic beliefs. Journal of Personal & Social Psychology, 601, 509–517. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Gilbert, D. T., Pelham, B. W., & Krull, D. S.
(1988) On cognitive busyness: When person perceivers meet persons perceived. Journal of Personal & Social Psychology, 541, 733–740. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Hassin, R. R., Aarts, H., & Ferguson, M. J.
(2005) Automatic goal inferences. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 411, 129–140. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Hilton, J. L., Fein, S., & Miller, D. T.
(1993) Suspicion and dispositional inference. Personality & Social Psychology Bulletin, 191, 501–512. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Kramer, R. M.
(1994) The sinister attribution error: Paranoid cognition and collective distrust in organizations. Motivation and Emotion, 181, 199–230. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Levine, T. R., & McCornack, S. A.
(1991) The dark side of trust: Conceptualizing and measuring types of communicative suspicion. Communication Quarterly, 391, 325–340. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Lynch, J. S., & van den Broek, P.
(2007) Understanding the glue of narrative structure: Children’s on- and off-line inferences about characters’ goals. Cognitive Development, 221, 323–340. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Magliano, J. P., Taylor, H. A., & Kim, H.-J. J.
(2005) When goals collide: Monitoring the goals of multiple characters. Memory & Cognition, 331, 1357–1367. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Main, K. J., Dahl, D. W., & Darke, P. R.
(2007) Deliberative and automatic bases of suspicion: Empirical evidence of the sinister attribution error. Journal of Consumer Psychology, 171, 59–69. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Malamuth, N. M., & Brown, L. M.
(1994) Sexually aggressive men’s perceptions of women’s communications: Testing three explanations. Journal of Personality & Social Psychology, 671, 699–712. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Marchand, M. A. G., & Vonk, R.
(2005) The process of becoming suspicious of ulterior motives. Social Cognition, 231, 242–256. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
McCornack, S. A., & Levine, T. R.
(1990) When lovers become leery: The relationship between and accuracy in detecting deception. Communication Monographs, 571, 219–230. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Meyer, J. R.
(1997) Cognitive influences on the ability to address interaction goals. In J. O. Greene (Ed.), Message production: Advances in communication theory (pp. 71–90). Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.Google Scholar
Nauta, A., & Sanders, K.
(2001) Causes and consequences of perceived goal differences between departments within manufacturing organizations. Journal of Occupational & Organizational Psychology, 741, 321–342. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Osborne, R. E., & Gilbert, D. T.
(1992) The preoccupational hazards of social life. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 621, 219–228. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Palomares, N. A.
(2008) Toward a theory of goal detection in social interaction: Effects of contextual ambiguity and tactical functionality on goal inferences and inference certainty. Communication Research, 351, 109–148. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
(2009a) Did you see it coming? Effects of the specificity and efficiency of goal pursuit on the accuracy and onset of goal detection in social interaction. Communication Research, 361, 475–509. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
(2009b) It’s not just your goal, but also who you know: How the cognitive associations among goals and relationships influence goal detection in social interaction. Human Communication Research, 351, 534–560. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
(2011) The dynamics of goal congruency and cognitive busyness in goal detection. Communication Research, 381, 517–542. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Poynor, D. V., & Morris, R. K.
(2003) Inferred goals in narratives: Evidence from self-paced reading, recall, and eye movements. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 291, 3–9.Google Scholar
Reid, V. M., Hoehl, S., Grigutsch, M., Groendahl, A., Parise, E., & Striano, T.
(2009) The neural correlates of infant and adult goal prediction: Evidence for semantic processing systems. Developmental Psychology, 451, 620–629. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Schank, R. C., & Abelson, R. P.
(1977) Scripts, plans, goals and understanding: An inquiry into human knowledge structures. Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.Google Scholar
Southgate, V., Johnson, M. H., & Csibra, G.
(2008) Infants attribute goals even to biomechanically impossible actions. Cognition, 1071, 1059–1069. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Svennevig, J.
(1999) Getting acquainted in conversation: A study of initial interactions. Philadelphia, PA: J. Benjamins Publications.Google Scholar
Tabachnick, B. G., & Fidell, L. S.
(2007) Using multivariate statistics, 5th ed. Boston, MA: Allyn and Bacon.Google Scholar
Uller, C.
(2004) Disposition to recognize goals in infant chimpanzees. Animal Cognition, 71, 154–161. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
West, S. G., Aiken, L. S., & Krull, J. L.
(1996) Experimental personality designs: Analyzing categorical by continuous variable interactions. Journal of Personality, 641, 1–48. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Wilensky, R.
(1983) Planning and understanding: A computational approach to human reasoning. London: Addison-Wesley.Google Scholar
Wilson, S. R.
(1990) Development and test of a cognitive rules model of interaction goals. Communication Monographs, 571, 81–103. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Wyer, R. S. Jr., & Srull, T. K.
(1989) Memory and cognition in its social context. Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.Google Scholar