Article published In:
Interaction and Iconicity in the Evolution of Language
Edited by Stefan Hartmann, Michael Pleyer, James Winters and Jordan Zlatev
[Interaction Studies 18:3] 2017
► pp. 376401
References
Ackermann, H., Hage, S. R., & Ziegler, W.
(2014) Brain mechanisms of acoustic communication in humans and nonhuman primates: An evolutionary perspective. Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 371, 529–604. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Ahlner, F., & Zlatev, J.
(2011) Cross-modal iconicity: A cognitive semiotic approach to sound symbolism. Sign Systems Studies, 381, 298–348. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Arbib, M. A., Liebal, K., & Pika, S.
(2008) Primate vocalization, gesture, and the evolution of human language. Current Anthropology, 491, 1053–1076. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Armstrong, D. F., & Wilcox, S. E.
(2007) The gestural origin of language. Oxford ; New York: Oxford University Press. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Bates, E.
(1976) Language and context: The acquisition of pragmatics. New York: Academic Press.Google Scholar
Bender, R., & Bender, N.
(2013) Brief communication: Swimming and diving behavior in apes (Pan troglodytes and Pongo pygmaeus): First documented report: Swimming and Diving behavior in Apes. American Journal of Physical Anthropology, 152(1), 156–162. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Bentley, M., & Varon, E. J.
(1933) An accessory study of phonetic symbolism. American Journal of Psychology, 451, 76–86. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Blackwell, N. L., Perlman, M., & Fox Tree, J. E.
(2015) Quotation as a multimodal construction. Journal of Pragmatics, 811, 1–7. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Blasi, D. E., Wichmann, S., Hammarstrom, H., Stadler, P. F., & Christiansen, M. H.
(2016) Sound-meaning association biases evidenced across thousands of languages. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences .1131, 10818–10823. DOI logo
Bonvillian, J. D., Orlansky, M. D., & Novack, L. L.
(1983) Developmental milestones: Sign language acquisition and motor development. Child Development, 541, 1435–1445. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Bremner, A. J., Caparos, S., Davidoff, J., de Fockert, J., Linnell, K. J. & Spence, C.
(2013) ‘‘Bouba’’ and ‘‘Kiki’’ in Namibia? A remote culture make similar shape–sound matches, but different shape–taste matches to Westerners. Cognition, 1261, 165–172. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Call, J., & Tomasello, M
(Eds.) (2007) The gestural communication of apes and monkeys. London: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates, Publishers.Google Scholar
Cartmill, E. A., Beilock, S., & Goldin-Meadow, S.
(2012) A word in the hand: Action, gesture and mental representation in humans and non-human primates. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society B, 3671, 29–143. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Clark, H. H.
(2016) Depicting as a method of communication. Psychological Review, 123(3), 324–347. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Clark, H. H., & Gerrig, R. J.
(1990) Quotations as demonstrations. Language, 66(4), 764. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Clark, N., Perlman, M., & Johansson Falck, M.
(2014) Iconic pitch expresses vertical space. In Language and the creative mind (pp. 393–410). Stanford: SCLI Publications.Google Scholar
Corballis, M. C.
(2003) From hand to mouth: The origins of language. Princeton, NJ; Woodstock: Princeton University Press.Google Scholar
Crockford, C., Herbinger, I., Vigilant, L., & Boesch, C.
(2004) Wild chimpanzees produce group-specific calls: A case for vocal learning? Ethology, 1101, 221–243. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Cuskley, C.
(2013) Mappings between linguistic sound and motion. Public Journal of Semiotics, 51, 39–62. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Dingemanse, M.
(2012) Advances in the cross-linguistic study of ideophones: Advances in the cross-linguistic study of ideophones. Language and Linguistics Compass, 6(10), 654–672. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
(2013) Ideophones and gesture in everyday speech. Gesture, 131, 146–165. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Dingemanse, M., Blasi, D. E., Lupyan, G., Christansen, M. H., & Monaghan, P.
(2015) Arbitrariness, iconicity, and systematicity in language. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 191, 603–615. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Douglas, P. H., & Moscovice, L. R.
(2015) Pointing and pantomime in wild apes? Female bonobos use referential and iconic gestures to request genito-genital rubbing. Scientific Reports, 5, 13999. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Evans, N. & Levinson, S. C.
(2009) The myth of language universals: Language diversity and its importance for cognitive science. Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 321, 429–448. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Fay, N., Arbib, M., & Garrod, S.
(2013) How to Bootstrap a Human Communication System. Cognitive Science, 37(7), 1356–1367. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Fay, N., Lister, C. J., Ellison, T. M., & Goldin-Meadow, S.
(2014) Creating a communication system from scratch: Gesture beats vocalization hands down. Frontiers in Psychology, 51, 1–12. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Feyereisen, P., & de Lannoy, J. D.
(1991) Gestures and speech: Psychological investigations. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Fitch, W. T.
(2010) The evolution of language. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Fouts, R. S., & Tukel, S.
(1997) Next of kin: My conversations with chimpanzees. New York: William Morrow.Google Scholar
Furness, W. H.
(1916) Observations on the mentality of chimpanzees and orang-utans. Proceedings of the American Philosophical Society , 551, 281–290.
Genty, E., & Zuberbühler, K.
(2015) Iconic gesturing in bonobos. Communicative & Integrative Biology, 81, e992742. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Goldin-Meadow, S.
(2003) The resilience of language: what gesture creation in deaf children can tell us about how all children learn language. New York, NY: Psychology Press.Google Scholar
(2016) What the hands can tell us about language emergence. Psychonomic Bulletin and Review. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Goodall, J.
(1986) The chimpanzees of Gombe: Patterns of behavior. Cambridge: Harvard University Press.Google Scholar
Graham, K. E., Furuichi, T., & Byrne, R. W.
(2016) The gestural repertoire of the wild bonobo (Pan paniscus): A mutually understood communication system. Animal Cognition. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Greenberg, J. H., & Jenkins, J. J.
(1966) Studies in the psychological correlates of the sound system of American English. Word, 221, 207–242. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Haiman, J.
(1980) The iconicity of grammar: Isomorphism and motivation. Language, 561, 515–540. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Hardus, M. E., Lameira, A. R., Van Schaik, C. P., & Wich, S. A.
(2009) Tool use in wild orang-utans modifies sound production: A functionally deceptive innovation? Proceedings of the Royal Society B , 2761, 3689–3694.
Harnad, S.
(1990) The symbol grounding problem. Physica D: Nonlinear Phenomena, 42(1-3), 335–346. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Hayes, C.
(1951) The ape in our house. New York: Harper.Google Scholar
Hewes, G. W.
(1973) Primate communication and the gestural origin of language. Current Anthropology, 14(1/2), 5–24. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Hobaiter, C. & Byrne, R. W.
(2011) The gestural repertoire of the wild chimpanzee. Animal Cognition, 141, 745–767. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Hocket, C. F.
(1960) The origin of speech. Scientific American, 2031, 88–111. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Hirata, S., Ukita, J., & Kita, S.
(2011) Implicit phonetic symbolism in voicing of consonants and visual lightness using Garner’s speeded classification task. Perceptual & Motor Skills, 1131, 929–940. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Hockett, C. F.
(1978) In search of Jove’s Brow. American Speech, 531, 243–313. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
(1987) Refurbishing our foundations. Amsterdam: John Benjamins. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Hopkins, W. D., & Savage-Rumbaugh, S.
(1991) Vocal communication as a function of differential rearing experiences in Pan paniscus: A preliminary report. International Journal of Primatology, 121, 559–583. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Imai, M., & Kita, S.
(2014) The sound symbolism bootstrapping hypothesis for language acquisition and language evolution. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences, 369(1651), 20130298. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Iverson, J. M., & Goldin-Meadow, S.
(1998) Why people gesture when they speak. Nature, 3961, 228–229. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Jespersen, O.
(1933) Symbolic value of the vowel i. In O. Jespersen (Ed.), Linguistica (pp. 283–303). Copenhagen: Levin & Munksgaard.Google Scholar
Joseph, J. E.
Kearton, C.
(1925) My friend Toto: The adventures of a chimpanzee, and the story of his journey from the Congo to London. London: Arrowsmith.Google Scholar
Kellogg, W. N., & Kellogg, L. A.
(1933) The ape and the child: A study of environmental influence upon early behavior. Oxford: Whittelsey House. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Kelly, B. F., Leben, W., & Cohen, R.
(2003) The meanings of consonants. Proceedings of the 29 th Berkeley Linguistics Society (pp. 245–253).
Kendon, A.
(1991) Some considerations for a theory of language origins. Man, (N. S.) 261, 602–619. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Klink, R. R.
(2000) Creating brand names with meaning: The use of sound symbolism. Marketing Letters, 111, 5–20. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Köhler, W.
(1929) Gestalt psychology. New York: Liveright.Google Scholar
Laidler, K.
(1980) The talking ape. New York: Stein and Day.Google Scholar
Lameira, A. R., Hardus, M. E., Kowalsky, B., de Vries, H., Spruijt, B. M., Sterck, E. H. M., Shumaker, R. W., & Wich, S. A.
(2013) Orangutan (Pongo spp.) whistling and implications for the emergence of an open-ended call repertoire: A replication and extension. Journal of the Acoustical Society of America, 1341, 2326–2335. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Lameira, A. R., Hardus, M. E., Bartlett, A. M., Shumaker, R. W., Wich, S. A., & Menken, S. B. J.
(2015) Speech-like rhythm in a voiced and voiceless orangutan call. PLoS ONE, 101, e116136. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Lameira, A. R., Hardus, M. E., Mielke, A., Wich, S. A., & Shumaker, R. W.
(2016) Vocal fold control beyond the species-specific repertoire in an orang-utan. Scientific Reports, 6, 30315. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Lameira, A. R., Maddieson, I., & Zuberbühler, K.
(2014) Primate feedstock for the evolution of consonants. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 18(2), 60–62. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Laporte, M. N. C., & Zuberbühler, K.
(2010) Vocal greeting behavior in wild chimpanzee females. Animal Behaviour, 801, 467–473. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Leavens, D. A.
(2003) Integration of visual and vocal communication: Evidence for Miocene origins. Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 261, 232–233. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Leavens, D. A., Russell, J. L., & Hopkins, W. D.
(2010) Multimodal communication by captive chimpanzees (Pan troglodytes). Animal Cognition, 131, 33–40. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Levinson, S. C., & Holler, J.
(2014) The origin of human multi-modal communication. Philosophical Transactions Royal Society B, 3691, 20130302. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Lewis, M., & Frank, M.
(2016) The length of words reflects their conceptual complexity. Cognition, 1531, 182–195. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Liebal, K., Waller, B., Burrows, A., & Slocombe, K. E.
(2013) Primate communication: A multimodal approach. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Lockwood, G., & Dingemanse, M.
(2015) Iconicity in the lab: A review of behavioral, developmental, and neuroimaging research into sound-symbolism. Frontiers in Psychology, 6, 1246.Google Scholar
Lupyan, G., & Casasanto, D.
(2014) Meaningless words promote meaningful categorization. Language and Cognition, 1–27.Google Scholar
MacLarnon, A. M., & Hewitt, G. P.
(1999) The evolution of human speech: The role of enhanced breathing control. American Journal of Physical Anthropology, 1091, 341–363. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Maddieson, I.
(2011) Consonant-vowel ratio. In The World Atlas of Language Stuctures Online. Max Planck Digital Library.Google Scholar
Marshall, A. J., Wrangham, R. W., & Arcadi, A. C.
(1999) Does learning affect the structure of vocalizations in chimpanzees? Animal Behaviour, 581, 825–830. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
McNeill, D.
(2012) How language began: Gesture and speech in human evolution. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Miles, L.
(1990) The cognitive foundations for reference in a signing orangutan. In S. T. Parker & K. R. Gibson (Eds.), “Language” and intelligence in monkeys and apes (pp. 511–539). Cambridge: Cambridge University Press. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Mitani, J. C., & Gros-Louis, J.
(1998) Chorusing and call convergence in chimpanzees: Tests of three hypotheses. Behaviour, 1351, 1041–1064. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Moos, A., Smith, R., Miller, S. R., & Simmons, D. R.
(2014) Cross-modal associations in synaesthesia: vowel colours in the ear of the beholder. i-Perception, 51, 132–142. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Newman, S. S.
(1933) Further experiments in phonetic symbolism. American Journal of Psychology, 451, 53–75. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Newmeyer, F. J.
(1992) Iconicity and generative grammar. Language, 681, 756–796. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Ohala, J. J.
(1994) The frequency code underlies the sound symbolic use of voice pitch. In L. Hinton, J. Nichols, & J. J. Ohala (Eds.), Sound Symbolism (pp. 325–347). Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Owren, M. J., Amoss, R. T., & Rendall, D.
(2011) Two organizing principles of vocal production: Implications for nonhuman and human primates. American Journal of Primatology, 731, 530–544. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Patterson, F. G., & Linden, E.
(1981) The education of Koko. New York: Holt, Rinhart & Winston.Google Scholar
Perlman, M., & Clark, N.
(2015) Learned vocal and breathing behavior in an enculturated gorilla. Animal Cognition, 181, 1165–1179. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Perlman, M., Clark, N., & Johansson Falck, M.
(2015) Iconic prosody in story reading. Cognitive Science, 39(6), 1348–1368. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Perlman, M., Dale, R., & Lupyan, G.
(2015) Iconicity can ground the creation of vocal symbols. Royal Society Open Science, 2(8), 150152. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Perlman, M., & Gibbs, R. W. Jr
(2013) Pantomimic gestures reveal the sensorimotor imagery of a human-fostered gorilla. Journal of Mental Imagery, 371, 73–96.Google Scholar
Perlman, M., & Lupyan, G.
(2015) The vocal iconicity challenge! Paper presented at Protolang 4 conference , Rome, Italy.
Perlman, M., Patterson, F. G., & Cohn, R. H.
(2012) The human-fostered gorilla Koko shows breath control in play with wind instruments. Biolinguistics, 61, 433–444.Google Scholar
Perlman, M., Paul, J. Z., & Lupyan, G.
(2015) Congenitally deaf children produce iconic vocalizations to communicate magnitude. In D. C. Noelle, R. Dale, A. S. Warlaumont, J. Yoshimi, T. Matlock, C. D. Jennings, & P. P. Maglio (Eds.), Proceedings of the 37th Annual Conference of the Cognitive Science Society (pp. 1853–1856). Austin, TX: Cognitive Science Society.Google Scholar
Perlman, M., Paul, J. Z. & Lupyan, G.
under review). Deaf and hearing Chinese children improvise iconic vocalizations that communicate magnitude to American listeners.
Perlman, M., Tanner, J. E., & King, B. J.
(2012) A mother gorilla’s variable use of touch to guide her infant: Insights into iconicity and the relationship between gesture and action. In S. Pika & K. Liebal (Eds.), Developments in primate gesture research (pp. 55–72). Amsterdam: John Benjamins. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Perniss, P., & Vigliocco, G.
(2014) The bridge of iconicity: From a world of experience to the experience of language. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences, 369, 20130300. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Perry, L. K., Perlman, M., & Lupyan, G.
(2015) Iconicity in English and Spanish and its relation to lexical category and age of acquisition. PLoS ONE, 10(9), e0137147. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Pika, S.
(2014) Chimpanzee grooming gestures and sounds: What might they tell us about how language evolved. In D. Dor, C. Knight, & J. Lewis (Eds.), The Social Origins of Language (pp. 128–140). Oxford: Oxford University Press. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Pinker, S.
(1994) The language instinct. New York: William Morrow & Co. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Pisanki, K., Cartei, V., McGettigan, C., Raine, J., & Reby, D.
(2016) Voice modulation: A window into the origins of human vocal control? Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 201, 304–318. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Ramachandran, V. S., & Hubbard, E. M.
(2001) Synaesthesia: A window into perception, thought and language. Journal of Consciousness Studies, 81, 3–34.Google Scholar
Russon, A., & Andrews, K.
(2010) Orangutan pantomime: Elaborating the message. Biology Letters, 7(4), 627–630. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Sandler, W., Meir, I., Padden, C., & Aronoff, M.
(2005) The emergence of grammar: systematic structure in a new language. Proceediings of the National Aacademy of Sciences, 1021, 2661–2665. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Sapir, E.
(1929) A study in phonetic symbolism. Journal of Experimental Psychology, 121, 225–239. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Saussure, Ferdinand de
([1916] 1966) Course in general linguistics. New York: McGraw-Hill.Google Scholar
Schel, A. M., Townsend, S. W., Machanda, Z., Zuberbühler, K, & Slocombe, K. E.
(2013) Chimpanzee Alarm Call Production Meets Key Criteria for Intentionality. PLoS ONE, 8(10), e76674. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Senghas, A., Kita, S., & zuryek, A.
(2004) Children creating core properties of language: evidence from an emergining sign language in Nicaragua. Science, 3051, 1779–1782. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Shintel, H., Nusbaum, H. C., & Okrent, A.
(2006) Analog acoustic expression in speech communication. Journal of Memory and Language, 55(2), 167–177. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Simner, J., Cuskley, C., & Kirby, S.
(2010) What sound does that taste? Cross-modal mappings across gustation and audition. Perception, 391, 553–569. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Skinner, B. F.
(1957) Verbal behavior. New York: Appleton-Century-Crofts. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Taglialatela, J. P., Savage-Rumbaugh, S., & Baker, L. A.
(2003) Vocal production by a language-competent Pan paniscus . International Journal of Primatology, 241, 1–17. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Taglialatela, J. P., Russell J. L., Schaeffer J. A., Hopkins, W. D.
(2011) Chimpanzee vocal signaling points to a multimodal origin of human language. PLoS One, 61, e18852. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Tanner, J. E., & Byrne, R. W.
(1996) Representation of action through iconic gesture in a captive lowland gorilla. Current Anthropology, 371, 162–173. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Thompson, P. D., & Estes, Z.
(2011) Sound symbolic naming of novel objects is a graded function. The Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 641, 2392–2404. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Tomasello, M.
(2008) Origins of human communication. Cambridge: MIT Press. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Van Schaik, C. P., Van Noordwijk, M. & Wich, S. A.
(2006) Innovation in wild Bornean orangutans (Pongo pygmaeus wurmbii). Behaviour, 1431, 839–876. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Watson, S. K., Townsend, S. W., Schel, A. M., Wilke, C., Wallace, E. K., Cheng, L., West, V., & Slocombe, K. E.
(2015) Vocal learning in the functionally referential food grunts of chimpanzees. Current Biology, 251, 495–499. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Wich, S. A., Swartz, K. B., Hardus, M. E., Lameira, A. R., Stromberg, E., & Shumaker, R. W.
(2009) A case of spontaneous acquisition of a human sound by an orangutan. Primates, 501, 56–64. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Wilson, M. L., Hauser, M. D., & Wrangham, R. W.
(2001) Does participation in intergroup conflict depend on numerical assessment, range location, or rank for wild chimpanzees? Animal Behaviour, 611, 1203–1216. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Witmer, L.
(1909) A monkey with a mind. The Psychological Clinic, III1, 179–205.Google Scholar
Cited by

Cited by 23 other publications

Bergman, Thore J., Jacinta C. Beehner, Melissa C. Painter & Morgan L. Gustison
2019. The speech-like properties of nonhuman primate vocalizations. Animal Behaviour 151  pp. 229 ff. DOI logo
Boeckx, Cedric
2023. What made us “hunter-gatherers of words”. Frontiers in Neuroscience 17 DOI logo
Dellert, Johannes, Niklas Erben Johansson, Johan Frid & Gerd Carling
2021. Preferred sound groups of vocal iconicity reflect evolutionary mechanisms of sound stability and first language acquisition: evidence from Eurasia. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences 376:1824 DOI logo
DINGEMANSE, MARK, MARCUS PERLMAN & PAMELA PERNISS
2020. Construals of iconicity: experimental approaches to form–meaning resemblances in language. Language and Cognition 12:1  pp. 1 ff. DOI logo
Erben Johansson, Niklas, Jon W Carr & Simon Kirby
2021. Cultural evolution leads to vocal iconicity in an experimental iterated learning task. Journal of Language Evolution 6:1  pp. 1 ff. DOI logo
Fröhlich, Marlen, Christine Sievers, Simon W. Townsend, Thibaud Gruber & Carel P. van Schaik
2019. Multimodal communication and language origins: integrating gestures and vocalizations. Biological Reviews 94:5  pp. 1809 ff. DOI logo
Graham, Kirsty E., Catherine Hobaiter, James Ounsley, Takeshi Furuichi, Richard W. Byrne & Lars Chittka
2018. Bonobo and chimpanzee gestures overlap extensively in meaning. PLOS Biology 16:2  pp. e2004825 ff. DOI logo
Hartmann, Stefan & Michael Pleyer
2021. Constructing a protolanguage: reconstructing prehistoric languages in a usage-based construction grammar framework. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences 376:1824 DOI logo
Hodge, Gabrielle
2020. Book review. Journal of Pragmatics 157  pp. 145 ff. DOI logo
Hodge, Gabrielle & Lindsay Ferrara
2022. Iconicity as Multimodal, Polysemiotic, and Plurifunctional. Frontiers in Psychology 13 DOI logo
Holler, Judith
2022. Visual bodily signals as core devices for coordinating minds in interaction. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences 377:1859 DOI logo
Kilpatrick, Alexander, Aleksandra Ćwiek, Eleanor Lewis & Shigeto Kawahara
2023. A cross-linguistic, sound symbolic relationship between labial consonants, voiced plosives, and Pokémon friendship. Frontiers in Psychology 14 DOI logo
Leongómez, Juan David, Jan Havlíček & S. Craig Roberts
2022. Musicality in human vocal communication: an evolutionary perspective. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences 377:1841 DOI logo
Macuch Silva, Vinicius, Judith Holler, Asli Ozyurek & Seán G. Roberts
2020. Multimodality and the origin of a novel communication system in face-to-face interaction. Royal Society Open Science 7:1  pp. 182056 ff. DOI logo
Markov, Ilia, Kseniia Kharitonova & Elena L. Grigorenko
2023. Language: Its Origin and Ongoing Evolution. Journal of Intelligence 11:4  pp. 61 ff. DOI logo
Mertz, Justine, Annaëlle Surreault, Erica van de Waal & Jennifer Botting
2019. Primates are living links to our past: The contribution of comparative studies with wild vervet monkeys to the field of social cognition. Cortex 118  pp. 65 ff. DOI logo
Mudd, Katie, Connie de Vos & Bart de Boer
2022. Shared Context Facilitates Lexical Variation in Sign Language Emergence. Languages 7:1  pp. 31 ff. DOI logo
Perniss, Pamela
2018. Why We Should Study Multimodal Language. Frontiers in Psychology 9 DOI logo
Pleyer, Michael, Ryan Lepic & Stefan Hartmann
2022. Compositionality in Different Modalities: A View from Usage-Based Linguistics. International Journal of Primatology DOI logo
Rasenberg, Marlou, Asli Özyürek, Sara Bögels & Mark Dingemanse
2022. The Primacy of Multimodal Alignment in Converging on Shared Symbols for Novel Referents. Discourse Processes 59:3  pp. 209 ff. DOI logo
Ćwiek, Aleksandra, Susanne Fuchs, Christoph Draxler, Eva Liina Asu, Dan Dediu, Katri Hiovain, Shigeto Kawahara, Sofia Koutalidis, Manfred Krifka, Pärtel Lippus, Gary Lupyan, Grace E. Oh, Jing Paul, Caterina Petrone, Rachid Ridouane, Sabine Reiter, Nathalie Schümchen, Ádám Szalontai, Özlem Ünal-Logacev, Jochen Zeller, Bodo Winter & Marcus Perlman
2021. Novel vocalizations are understood across cultures. Scientific Reports 11:1 DOI logo
Żywiczyński, Przemysław
2019. How research on language evolution contributes to linguistics. Yearbook of the Poznan Linguistic Meeting 5:1  pp. 1 ff. DOI logo
Żywiczyński, Przemysław, Sławomir Wacewicz & Casey Lister
2021. Pantomimic fossils in modern human communication. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences 376:1824 DOI logo

This list is based on CrossRef data as of 31 march 2024. Please note that it may not be complete. Sources presented here have been supplied by the respective publishers. Any errors therein should be reported to them.