Article published In:
Interaction Studies
Vol. 22:1 (2021) ► pp.86109
References
Alimardani, M., Nishio, S., & Ishiguro, H.
(2013) Humanlike robot hands controlled by brain activity arouse illusion of ownership in operators. Scientific reports, 31, 2396. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
(2014) Effect of biased feedback on motor imagery learning in BCI-teleoperation system. Frontiers in systems neuroscience, 81, 52. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Arimoto, T., Yoshikawa, Y., & Ishiguro, H.
(2018) Multiple-Robot Conversational Patterns for Concealing Incoherent Responses. International Journal of Social Robotics, 1–11. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Avrunin, E., Hart, J., Douglas, A., & Scassellati, B.
(2011, March). Effects related to synchrony and repertoire in perceptions of robot dance. In Proceedings of the 6th international conference on Human-robot interaction (pp. 93–100). ACM. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Barsade, S. G.
(2002) The ripple effect: Emotional contagion and its influence on group behavior. Administrative Science Quarterly, 47(4), 644–675. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Bhide, A., Power, A., & Goswami, U.
(2013) A rhythmic musical intervention for poor readers: A comparison of efficacy with a letter-based intervention. Mind, Brain, and Education, 7(2), 113–123. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Blakemore et al.
(1999) Spatio-Temporal Prediction Modulates the Perception of Self-Produced Stimuli, Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 11:5, pp.551–559. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Botvinick, M., & Cohen, J.
(1998) Rubber hands ‘feel’ touch that eyes see. Nature, 391(6669), 756. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Briggs, G., & Scheutz, M.
(2014) How robots can affect human behavior: Investigating the effects of robotic displays of protest and distress. International Journal of Social Robotics, 6(3), 343–355. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Brown, S.
(2000) Evolutionary models of music: From sexual selection to group selection. In Perspectives in ethology (pp. 231–281). Springer, Boston, MA. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Cohen, G. L.
(2003) Party over policy: The dominating impact of group influence on political beliefs. Journal of personality and social psychology, 85(5), 808. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Crick, C., Munz, M., & Scassellati, B.
(2006, September). Synchronization in social tasks: Robotic drumming. In Robot and Human Interactive Communication, 2006. ROMAN 2006. (pp. 97–102). The 15th IEEE International Symposium. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
David, D., Wade-Woolley, L., Kirby, J., Smithrim, K.
(2007) Rhythm and reading development in school-age children: A longitudinal study. Journal of Research in Reading. Volume 30, Issue 2, 169–183. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Eerola, T., Luck, G., & Toiviainen, P.
(2006, August). An investigation of pre-schoolers’ corporeal synchronization with music. In Proceedings of the 9th international conference on music perception and cognition (pp. 472–476). The Society for Music Perception and Cognition and European Society for the Cognitive Sciences of Music Bologna.Google Scholar
Gan, L., Huang, Y., Zhou, L., Qian, C., & Wu, X.
(2015) Synchronization to a bouncing ball with a realistic motion trajectory. Scientific reports, 51, 11974. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Hoffman, G., & Vanunu, K.
(2013) Effects of Robotic Companionship on Music Enjoyment and Agent Perception. In Proceedings of the 3rd ACM/IEEE International Conference on Human-Robot Interaction (pp. 317–324). IEEE/RSJ International Conference on Intelligent Robots and Systems Int’l Workshop on Human-Agent Interaction. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Hove, M. J., Iversen, J. R., Zhang, A., & Repp, B. H.
(2013) Synchronization with competing visual and auditory rhythms: bouncing ball meets metronome. Psychological Research, 77(4), 388–398. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Iio, T., Yoshikawa, Y., & Ishiguro, H.
(2016, October). Pre-scheduled turn-taking between robots to make conversation coherent. In Proceedings of the Fourth International Conference on Human Agent Interaction (pp. 19–25). ACM. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Iqbal, T., Moosaei, M., & Riek, L. D.
(2016) Tempo adaptation and anticipation methods for human-robot teams. RSS, Planning for HRI: Shared Autonomy and Collab. Robotics Work.Google Scholar
Jaffe, J., Beebe, B., Feldstein, S., Crown, C. L., Jasnow, M. D., Rochat, P., & Stern, D. N.
(2001) Rhythms of dialogue in infancy: Coordinated timing in development. Monographs of the society for research in child development, i–149.Google Scholar
Kawasaki, M., Yamada, Y., Ushiku, Y., Miyauchi, E., & Yamaguchi, Y.
(2013) Inter-brain synchronization during coordination of speech rhythm in human-to-human social interaction. Scientific reports, 31, 1692. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Kelso, J. S.
(1997) Dynamic patterns: The self-organization of brain and behavior. MIT press.Google Scholar
Kose-Bagci, H., Dautenhahn, K., & Nehaniv, C. L.
(2008, August). Emergent dynamics of turn-taking interaction in drumming games with a humanoid robot. In RO-MAN 2008-The 17th IEEE International Symposium on Robot and Human Interactive Communication (pp. 346–353). IEEE. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
LaGasse, A. B., & Hardy, M. W.
(2013) Rhythm, movement, and autism: using rhythmic rehabilitation research as a model for autism. Frontiers in integrative neuroscience, 71, 19.Google Scholar
Lenggenhager, B., Tadi, T., Metzinger, T., & Blanke, O.
(2007) Video ergo sum: manipulating bodily self-consciousness. Science, 317(5841), 1096–1099. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Ma, K., & Hommel, B.
(2015) The role of agency for perceived ownership in the virtual hand illusion. Consciousness and cognition, 361, 277–288. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Mates, J.
(1994) A model of synchronization of motor acts to a stimulus sequence. Biological cybernetics, 70(5), 463–473. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
McNeill, W. H.
(2009) Keeping Together in Time: Dance and Drill in Human History. Harvard Univ. Press. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Merker, B.
(1999) Synchronous chorusing and the origins of music. Musicae Scientiae, 3(1_suppl), 59–73. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Michalowski, M. P., Sabanovic, S., & Kozima, H.
(2007, March). A dancing robot for rhythmic social interaction. In 2nd ACM/IEEE International Conference on Human-Robot Interaction (HRI), 2007 (pp. 89–96). IEEE. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Michalowski, M. P., Simmons, R., & Kozima, H.
(2009, September). Rhythmic attention in child-robot dance play. In Robot and Human Interactive Communication, 2009. RO-MAN 2009. The 18th IEEE International Symposium on (pp. 816–821). IEEE. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Miles, L. K., Nind, L. K., & Macrae, C. N.
(2009) The rhythm of rapport: Interpersonal synchrony and social perception. Journal of experimental social psychology, 45(3), 585–589. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Milgram, S., Bickman, L., & Berkowitz, L.
(1969) Note on the drawing power of crowds of different size. Journal of personality and social psychology, 13(2), 79. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Milgram, S. & Gudehus, C.
(1978) Obedience to authority.Google Scholar
Mörtl, A., Lorenz, T., & Hirche, S.
(2014) Rhythm patterns interaction-synchronization behavior for human-robot joint action. PloS one, 9(4), e95195. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Néda, Z., Ravasz, E., Brechet, Y., Vicsek, T., & Barabási, A. L.
(2000) Tumultuous applause can transform itself into waves of synchronized clapping. Nature, 403(6772), 849–850. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Nishio, S., Watanabe, T., Ogawa, K., & Ishiguro, H.
(2012, October). Body ownership transfer to teleoperated android robot. In International Conference on Social Robotics (pp. 398–407). Springer, Berlin, Heidelberg. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Overy, K.
(2012) Making music in a group: synchronization and shared experience. Annals of the New York Academy of Sciences, 1252(1), 65–68. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Pikovsky, A., Rosenblum, M., & Kurths, J.
(2003) Synchronization: a universal concept in nonlinear sciences (Vol. 121). Cambridge university press. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Rau, P. L. P., Li, Y., & Liu, J.
(2013) Effects of a social robot’s autonomy and group orientation on human decision-making. Advances in Human-Computer Interaction 2013, 111. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Richardson, M. J., Marsh, K. L., Isenhower, R. W., Goodman, J. R., & Schmidt, R. C.
(2007) Rocking together: Dynamics of intentional and unintentional interpersonal coordination. Human movement science, 26(6), 867–891. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Richardson, M. J., Marsh, K. L., & Schmidt, R. C.
(2005) Effects of visual and verbal interaction on unintentional interpersonal coordination. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 31(1), 62. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Roederer, J. G.
(1984) The search for a survival value of music. Music Perception: An Interdisciplinary Journal, 1(3), 350–356. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Schloss, W. A.
(1985) On the automatic transcription of percussive music – From acoustic signals to high-level analysis (Doctoral dissertation, Stanford University).
Schmidt, R. C., Carello, C., & Turvey, M. T.
(1990) Phase transitions and critical fluctuations in the visual coordination of rhythmic movements between people. Journal of experimental psychology: human perception and performance, 16(2), 227.Google Scholar
Scothern, P. M. T.
(1992) The music-archaeology of the Palaeolithic within its cultural setting (Doctoral dissertation, University of Cambridge).
Shimada et al.
(2009) Rubber Hand Illusion under Delayed Visual Feedback, PLoS ONE, 4(7): e6185. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Slater, M., Pérez–Marcos, D., Ehrsson, H., & Sanchez–Vives, M. V.
(2008) Towards a digital body: the virtual arm illusion. Frontiers in human neuroscience, 21, 6. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Tsakiris, M., & Haggard, P.
(2005) The rubber hand illusion revisited: visuotactile integration and self-attribution. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 31(1), 80.Google Scholar
Valdesolo, P., Ouyang, J., & DeSteno, D.
(2010) The rhythm of joint action: Synchrony promotes cooperative ability. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 46(4), 693–695. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
van Ulzen, N. R., Lamoth, C. J., Daffertshofer, A., Semin, G. R., & Beek, P. J.
(2008) Characteristics of instructed and uninstructed interpersonal coordination while walking side-by-side. Neuroscience letters, 432(2), 88–93. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Van Vugt, F. T., Ritter, J., Rollnik, J. D., & Altenmüller, E.
(2014) Music-supported motor training after stroke reveals no superiority of synchronization in group therapy. Frontiers in human neuroscience, 81, 315. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Wallach, M. A., Kogan, N., & Bem, D. J.
(1962) Group influence on individual risk taking. ETS Research Report Series 1962(1).Google Scholar
Wilder, D. A.
(1977) Perception of groups, size of opposition, and social influence. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 13(3), 253–268. DOI logoGoogle Scholar