Review published In:
ITL - International Journal of Applied Linguistics
Vol. 155 (2008) ► pp.125131
References
Chomsky, N.
(1995) The Minimalist Program. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.Google Scholar
Gess, R., & Herschensohn, J.
(2000) Shifting the DP parameter: A study of Anglophone French L2ers. Paper presented at the Linguistic Symposium on Romance Languages 30, Gainesville, Florida. Ms University of Utah / University of Washington.Google Scholar
Hawkins, R., & Chan, C. Y.-H.
(1997) The partial availability of Universal Grammar in second language acquisition: the ‘Failed Functional Features Hypothesis’. Second Language Research, 131, 187–226. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Lardière, D.
(1998) Dissociating syntax from morphology in a divergent end-state grammar. Second Language Research 141:359–375. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Prévost, P., & White, L.
(2000) Missing Surface Inflection or Impairment in second language acquisition? Evidence from tense and agreement. Second Language Research, 161, 103–133. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Schwartz, B.D., & Sprouse, R.
(1996) L2 cognitive states and the Full Transfer/Full Access model. Second Language Research, 121, 40–72. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
White, L., Valenzuela, E., Kozlowska-Macgregor, M., & Leung, I.
(2002) Gender agreement in non-native Spanish: Evidence against Failed Features. McGill Working Papers in Linguistics.Google Scholar
White, L., Valenzuela, E., Kozlowska-Macgregor, M., Leung, I., & Ben Ayed, H.
(2001) The status of abstract features in Interlanguage: Gender and number in L2 Spanish. In A. H.-J. Do, L. Domínguez & A. Johansen (Eds.), BUCLD 25 Proceedings pp.792–802. Somerville, MA: Cascadilla Press.Google Scholar