Article published In:
Journal of Second Language Pronunciation
Vol. 6:2 (2020) ► pp.209235
References (49)
References
Abramson, A. (1975). The Tones of Central Thai: Some Perceptual Experiments. In J. G. Harris & J. R. Chamberlain (Eds.), Studies in Thai Linguistics in Honor of William J.Gedney (pp. 1–16). Bangkok: Central Institute of English Language, Office of State Universities.Google Scholar
Alexander, J. A., Wong, P. C. M., & Bradlow, A. R. (2005). Lexical tone perception in musicians and non-musicians. In Ninth European Conference on Speech Communication and Technology.Google Scholar
Best, C. T., & Tyler, M. D. (2007). Nonnative and second-language speech perception: Commonalities and complementarities. Language Experience in Second Language Speech Learning: In Honor of James Emil Flege, 13341. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Boersma, P., & Weenink, D. (2017). Praat: doing phonetics by computer [Computer program]. Version 6.0.30, retrieved 22 July 2017 from [URL]
Chang, C. B., & Bowles, A. R. (2015). Context effects on second-language learning of tonal contrasts. The Journal of the Acoustical Society of America, 138(6), 3703–3716. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Chao, Y. R. (1965). A grammar of spoken Chinese. Univ of California Press.Google Scholar
Chen, Y., & Xu, Y. (2006). Production of Weak Elements in Speech--Evidence from F₀ Patterns of Neutral Tone in Standard Chinese. Phonetica, 63(1), 47–75. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Chuang, C. K., Hiki, S., Sone, T., & Nimura, T. (1972). The acoustical features and perceptual cues of the four tones of standard colloquial Chinese. In Proceedings of the 7th International Congress of Acoustics, vol. 3 (pp. 297–300). Budapest: Akademial Kiado.Google Scholar
Francis, A. L., Ciocca, V., Wong, N. K. Y., Leung, W. H. Y., & Chu, P. C. Y. (2006). Extrinsic context affects perceptual normalization of lexical tone. The Journal of the Acoustical Society of America, 119(3), 1712–1726. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Gandour, J. (1983). Tone perception in Far Eastern languages. Journal of Phonetics. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Gårding, E., Kratochvil, P., Svantesson, J.-O., & Zhang, J. (1986). Tone 4 and Tone 3 discrimination in modern standard Chinese. Language and Speech, 29(3), 281–293. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Gottfried, T. L., & Suiter, T. L. (1997). Effect of linguistic experience on the identification of Mandarin Chinese vowels and tones. Journal of Phonetics, 25(2), 207–231. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Hao, Y.-C. (2012). Second language acquisition of Mandarin Chinese tones by tonal and non-tonal language speakers. Journal of Phonetics, 40(2), 269–279. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Hockett, C. F. (1947). Peiping phonology. Journal of the American Oriental Society, 67(4), 253–267. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Howie, J. M. (1976). Acoustical studies of Mandarin vowels and tones (Vol. 61). Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Iverson, P., & Evans, B. G. (2009). Learning English vowels with different first-language vowel systems II: Auditory training for native Spanish and German speakers a. The Journal of the Acoustical Society of America, 126(2), 866–877. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Kuang, J. (2017). Covariation between voice quality and pitch: Revisiting the case of Mandarin creaky voice. The Journal of the Acoustical Society of America, 142(3), 1693–1706. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Lehiste, I. (1970). Suprasegmentals. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.Google Scholar
Levis, J. M. (2016). Research into practice: How research appears in pronunciation teaching materials. Language Teaching, 49(3), 423–437. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Li, C. (1987). Remarks on the Underlying Representations of Neutral-Tone Words in Mandarin Chinese. Stud. English Lit. Linguist, 189–199.Google Scholar
Li, C. N., & Thompson, S. A. (1989). Mandarin Chinese: A functional reference grammar. Univ of California Press.Google Scholar
Lin, M. C. (1965). The pitch indicator and the pitch characteristics of tones in Standard Chinese. Acta Acoustica (China), 21, 8–15.Google Scholar
Liu, Y. (2008). Integrated Chinese: Simplified Characters Textbook, Level 1, Part 1 (English and Chinese Edition) (3rd ed.). Cheng & Tsui.Google Scholar
Lively, S. E., Logan, J. S., & Pisoni, D. B. (1993). Training Japanese listeners to identify English /r/ and /l/. II: The role of phonetic environment and talker variability in learning new perceptual categories. The Journal of the Acoustical Society of America, 94(3), 1242–1255. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Lively, S. E., Pisoni, D. B., Yamada, R. A., Tohkura, Y., & Yamada, T. (1994). Training Japanese listeners to identify English /r/ and /l/. III. Long-term retention of new phonetic categories. The Journal of the Acoustical Society of America, 96(4), 2076–2087. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Logan, J. S., Lively, S. E., & Pisoni, D. B. (1991). Training Japanese listeners to identify English /r/ and /l/: A first report. The Journal of the Acoustical Society of America, 89(2), 874–886. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Moore, C. B., & Jongman, A. (1997). Speaker normalization in the perception of Mandarin Chinese tones. The Journal of the Acoustical Society of America, 102(3), 1864–1877. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Niebuhr, O., Alm, M., Schümchen, N., & Fischer, K. (2017). Comparing visualization techniques for learning second language prosody. International Journal of Learner Corpus Research, 3(2), 250–277. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Norman, J. (1988). Chinese. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Perrachione, T. K., Lee, J., Ha, L. Y. Y., & Wong, P. C. M. (2011). Learning a novel phonological contrast depends on interactions between individual differences and training paradigm design. The Journal of the Acoustical Society of America, 130(1), 461–472. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Pruitt, J. S., Jenkins, J. J., & Strange, W. (2006). Training the perception of Hindi dental and retroflex stops by native speakers of American English and Japanese. The Journal of the Acoustical Society of America, 119(3), 1684–1696. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Sadakata, M., & McQueen, J. M. (2013). High stimulus variability in nonnative speech learning supports formation of abstract categories: Evidence from Japanese geminates. The Journal of the Acoustical Society of America, 134(2), 1324–1335. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
(2014). Individual aptitude in Mandarin lexical tone perception predicts effectiveness of high-variability training. Frontiers in Psychology, 51. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Shen, X. S. (1989). Toward a register approach in teaching Mandarin tones. Journal of Chinese Language Teachers Association, 24(3), 27–47.Google Scholar
Shen, X. S., & Lin, M. (1991). Concept of tone in Mandarin revisited: A perceptual study on tonal coarticulation. Language Sciences, 13(3–4), 421–432. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Shen, X. S., Lin, M., & Yan, J. (1993). F 0 turning point as an F 0 cue to tonal contrast: a case study of Mandarin tones 2 and 3. The Journal of the Acoustical Society of America, 93(4), 2241–2243. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
So, C. K., & Best, C. T. (2010). Cross-language perception of non-native tonal contrasts: Effects of native phonological and phonetic influences. Language and Speech, 53(2), 273–293. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
The MathWorks. (2018). MATLAB (Version 9.5.0.944444) [Computer software]. Natick, MA: The MathWorks.Google Scholar
Thomson, R. I. (2018). High Variability [Pronunciation] Training (HVPT). Journal of Second Language Pronunciation, 4(2), 208–231. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Tseng, C. (1990). An Acoustic Phonetic Study on Tones in Mandarin Chinese. Nankang, Taipei: Institute of History & Philology, Academia Sinica. special publications.Google Scholar
Wang, X. (2013). Perception of Mandarin tones: The effect of L1 background and training. The Modern Language Journal, 97(1), 144–160. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Wang, Y., Spence, M. M., Jongman, A., & Sereno, J. A. (1999). Training American listeners to perceive Mandarin tones. The Journal of the Acoustical Society of America, 106(6), 3649–3658. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Wayland, R. P., & Guion, S. G. (2004). Training English and Chinese listeners to perceive Thai tones: A preliminary report. Language Learning, 54(4), 681–712. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Whalen, D. H., & Xu, Y. (1992). Information for Mandarin tones in the amplitude contour and in brief segments. Phonetica, 49(1), 25–47. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
White, C. M. (1981). Tonal perception errors and interference from English intonation. Journal of Chinese Language Teachers Association, 16(2), 27–56.Google Scholar
Wong, P. C. M., & Perrachione, T. K. (2007). Learning pitch patterns in lexical identification by native English-speaking adults. Applied Psycholinguistics, 28(04), 565–585. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Wong, P. C. M., Skoe, E., Russo, N. M., Dees, T., & Kraus, N. (2007). Musical experience shapes human brainstem encoding of linguistic pitch patterns. Nature Neuroscience, 10(4), 420. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Zhang, K., Peng, G., Li, Y., Minett, J. W., & Wang, W. S. Y. (2018). The effect of speech variability on tonal language speakers’ second language lexical tone learning. Frontiers in Psychology, 91, 1982. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Zhao, T. C., & Kuhl, P. K. (2015). Effect of musical experience on learning lexical tone categories. The Journal of the Acoustical Society of America, 137(3), 1452–1463. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Cited by (6)

Cited by six other publications

Bieber, Rebecca E., Matthew J. Makashay, Brian Simpson, Benjamin M. Sheffield & Douglas S. Brungart
2023. Short-term retention of learning after rapid adaptation to native and non-native speech. The Journal of the Acoustical Society of America 153:6  pp. 3362 ff. DOI logo
Naito, Yuka
2023. Does Musical Training Influence Perceptual Learning of Japanese Pitch Accent? The Case of Native Italian Speakers. In Advances in Second/Foreign Language Acquisition,  pp. 1 ff. DOI logo
Zhang, Xiaojuan, Bing Cheng, Yu Zou, Xujia Li & Yang Zhang
2023. Cognitive factors in nonnative phonetic learning: Impacts of inhibitory control and working memory on the benefits and costs of talker variability. Journal of Phonetics 100  pp. 101266 ff. DOI logo
Li, Xiaoshi, Qian Luo, Jie Liu & Catherine Ryu
2022. Varying Shades of Hearing. Researching and Teaching Chinese as a Foreign Language 3:2 DOI logo
O'Brien, Mary Grantham
2022. Making the Teaching of Suprasegmentals Accessible. In Second Language Pronunciation,  pp. 85 ff. DOI logo
Zhang, Xiaojuan, Bing Cheng & Yang Zhang
2021. The Role of Talker Variability in Nonnative Phonetic Learning: A Systematic Review and Meta-Analysis. Journal of Speech, Language, and Hearing Research 64:12  pp. 4802 ff. DOI logo

This list is based on CrossRef data as of 20 october 2024. Please note that it may not be complete. Sources presented here have been supplied by the respective publishers. Any errors therein should be reported to them.