Article published in:
Journal of Second Language Studies
Vol. 1:1 (2018) ► pp. 79105
References
Abu-Rabia, S.
(2001) Testing the interdependence hypothesis among native adult bilingual Russian-English students. Journal of Psycholinguistic Research, 30(4), 437–455. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Abu-Rabia, S., & Sanitsky, E.
(2010) Advantages of bilinguals over monolinguals in learning a third language. Bilingual Research Journal: The Journal of the National Association for Bilingual Education, 33(2), 173–199. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Akamatsu, N.
(2008) The effects of training on automatization of word recognition in English as a foreign language. Applied Psycholinguistics, 291, 175–193. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Antoniou, M., Liang, E., Ettlinger, M. & Wong, P. C. M.
(2015) The bilingual advantage in phonetic learning. Bilingualism: Language and Cognition, 181, 683–695. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Atkins, P. W. B., & Baddeley, A. D.
(1998) Working memory and distributed vocabulary learning. Applied Psycholinguistics, 191, 537–552. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Baddeley, A. D., & Hitch, G. J.
(1974) Working memory. In G. H. Bower (Ed.), The psychology of learning and motivation: Advances in research and theory (pp. 47–89). New York, NY: Academic Press.Google Scholar
Baddeley, A., Papagno, C., & Vallar, G.
(1988) When long-term learning depends on short-term storage. Journal of Memory and Language, 27(5), 586–595. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Baddeley, A., Gathercole, S., & Papagno, C.
(1998) The phonological loop as language learning device. Psychological Review, 1051, 158–173. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Bialystok, E., Craik, F. I. M., Klein, R., & Viswanathan, M.
(2004) Bilingualism, aging, and cognitive control: Evidence from the Simon task. Psychology and Aging, 19(2), 290–303. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Bialystok, E., Craik, F. I. M., & Ryan, J.
(2006) Executive control in a modified antisaccade task: Effects of aging and bilingualism. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory and Cognition, 321, 1341–1354.Google Scholar
Cenoz, J.
(2003) The additive effect of bilingualism on third language acquisition: A review. The International Journal of Bilingualism, 7(1), 71–87. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
(2013) The influence of bilingualism on third language acquisition: Focus on multilingualism. Language Teaching¸46, 71–86. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Cenoz, J., & Valencia, J. F.
(1994) Additive trilingualism: Evidence from the Basque country. Applied Psycholinguistics, 151, 195–207. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Costa, A., Hernández, M., & Sebastián-Gallés, N.
(2008) Bilingualism aids conflict resolution: Evidence from the ANT task. Cognition, 106(1), 59–86. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Craik, F.
(1986) A functional account of age differences in memory. In F. Klix & H. Hagendorf (Eds.), Human memory and cognitive abilities (pp. 409–422). Amsterdam: Elsevier.Google Scholar
Forster, K. I., & Forster, J. C.
(2003) DMDX: A Windows display program with millisecond accuracy. Behavior Research Methods, Instruments & Computers, 351, 116–124. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Gathercole, S., & Masoura, E.
(2003) Contrasting contributions of phonological short-term memory and long-term knowledge to vocabulary learning in a foreign language. Memory, 12(3–4), 422–429.Google Scholar
Gathercole, V. C. M., Thomas, E. M., Kennedy, I., Prys, C., Young, N., Viñas-Guasch, N., Roberts, E. J., Hughes, E. K., & Jones, L.
(2014) Does language dominance affect cognitive performance in bilinguals? Lifespan evidence from preschoolers through older adults on card sorting, Simon, and metalinguistic tasks. Frontiers in Psychology, 5(11), 1–14.Google Scholar
Green, D. W.
(1998) Mental control of the bilingual lexicosemantic system. Bilingualism: Language and Cognition, 11, 67–81. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Hernandez, A. E., & Meschyan, G.
(2006) Executive function is necessary to enhance lexical processing in a less proficient L2: Evidence from fMRI during picture naming. Bilingualism: Language and cognition, 9(2), 177–188. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Hilchey, M. D., & Klein, R. M.
(2011) Are there bilingual advantages on nonlinguistic interference tasks? Implications for the plasticity of executive control processes. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 18(4), 625–658. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Kaushanskaya, M., & Marian, V.
(2009) The bilingual advantage in novel word learning. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 16(4), 705–710. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Keshavarz, M. H. & Astaneh, H.
(2004) The impact of bilinguality on the learning of English vocabulary as a foreign language (L3). International Journal of Bilingual Education and Bilingualism, 7(4), 295–302. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Lado, B., Bowden, H. W., Stafford, C., & Sanz, C.
(2017) Two birds, one stone, or how learning a foreign language makes you a better language learner. Hispania, 100(3), 361–378. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Linck, J. A., & Weiss, D. J.
(2011) Working memory predicts the acquisition of explicit L2 knowledge. In C. Sanz & R. P. Leow (Eds.), Implicit and explicit language learning: Conditions, processes, and knowledge in SLA and bilingualism (pp. 101–114). Washington, DC: Georgetown University Press.Google Scholar
Linck, J. A., Osthus, P., Koeth, J. T., & Bunting, M. F.
(2014) Working memory and second language comprehension and production: A meta-analysis. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 21(4), 861–883. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Marian, V., Blumenfeld, H. K., & Kaushanskaya, M.
(2007) The language experience and proficiency questionnaire (LEAP-Q): Assessing language profiles in bilinguals and multilinguals. Journal of Speech, Language, and Hearing Research, 50(4), 940–967. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Martin, K., & Ellis, N.
(2012) The roles of phonological short-term memory and working memory in L2 grammar and vocabulary learning. Studies in Second Language Acquisition, 341, 379–413. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Nation, R., & McLaughlin, B.
(1986) Novices and experts: An information processing approach to the “good language learner” problem. Applied Psycholinguistics, 71, 41–56. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Nayak, N., Hansen, N., Krueger, N., & McLaughlin, B.
(1990) Language‐learning strategies in monolingual and multilingual adults. Language learning, 40(2), 221–244. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Paap, K. R., & Greenberg, Z. I.
(2013) There is no coherent evidence for a bilingual advantage in executive processing. Cognitive psychology, 66(2), 232–258. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Papagno, C., & Vallar, G.
(1995) Verbal short-term memory and vocabulary learning in polyglots. The Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology Section A: Human Experimental Psychology, 48(1), 98–107. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Papagno, G., Valentine, T., & Baddeley, A.
(1991) Phonological short-term memory and foreign-language vocabulary learning. Journal of Memory and Language, 301, 331–347. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Prior, M., & MacWhinney, B.
(2010) A bilingual advantage in task switching. Bilingualism: Language and Cognition, 13(2), 253–262. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Ratiu, I. & Azuma, T.
(2015) Working memory capacity: Is there a bilingual advantage? Journal of Cognitive Psychology, 27(1), 1–11. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Rossion, B., & Pourtois, G.
(2004) Revisiting Snodgrass and Vanderwart’s object set: The role of surface detail in basic-level object recognition. Perception, 331, 217–236. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Sanz, C.
(2000) Bilingual education enhances third language acquisition: Evidence from Catalonia. Applied Psycholinguistics, 211, 23–44. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Service, E.
(1992) Phonology, working memory, and foreign-language learning. The Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Experimental Psychology, 45(1), 21–50. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Soveri, A., Laine, M., Hamalainen, H., & Hugdahl, K.
(2011) Bilingual advantage in attentional control: Evidence from the forced-attention dichotic listening paradigm. Bilingualism: Language and Cognition, 14(3), 371–378. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Speciale, G., Ellis, N. C., & Bywater, T.
(2004) Phonological sequence learning and short-term store capacity determine second language vocabulary acquisition. Applied Psycholinguistics, 251, 293–321. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Stafford, C.
(2011) Bilingualism and enhanced attention in early adulthood. International Journal of Bilingual Education and Bilingualism, 14(1), 1–22. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Thomas, J.
(1988) The role played by metalinguistic awareness in second and third language learning. Journal of Multilingual and Multicultural Development, 9(3), 235–246. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Vallar, G. & Papagno, C.
(2002) Neuropsychological impairments of verbal short-term memory. In A. D. Baddeley, M. D. Kopelman, & B. A. Wilson (Eds.), The handbook of memory disorders (2nd ed.; pp. 249–270). Chichester, UK: Wiley & Sons.Google Scholar
Van Hell, J. G., & Candia Mahn, A.
(1997) Keyword mnemonics versus rote rehearsal: Learning concrete and abstract foreign words by experience and inexperienced learners. Language learning, 47(3), 507–546. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Weinbach, N., & Henik, A.
(2012) Temporal orienting and alerting – same or different? Frontiers in Psychology, 31, 1–3. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Williams, J. N.
(2011) Working memory and SLA. In S. M. Gass & A. Mackey (Eds.), The handbook of second language acquisition (pp. 427–441). New York, NY: Routledge.Google Scholar
Yang, H., Yang, S., Ceci, S., & Wang, Q.
(2005) Effects of bilinguals’ controlled-attention on working memory and recognition. In J. Cohen, K. T. McAlister, K. Rolstad, & J. MacSwan (Eds.), ISB4: Proceedings of the 4th International Symposium on Bilingualism (pp. 2401–2404). Somerville, MA: Cascadilla Press.Google Scholar
Effects of second language proficiency and working memory span on novel language learning