This paper reports on a study investigating the role of working memory in predicting L2 development under
immediate and delayed corrective feedback (CF) conditions. A total of 106 seventh-grade EFL learners were assigned to three
groups: Immediate CF, Delayed CF, and Task Only. Each group underwent three treatment sessions during which they performed six
focused communicative tasks – two in each session – involving the use of the English past tense. The Immediate CF group received
feedback on their erroneous use of the target structure during their task performance in Session 1; the Delayed CF group did not
receive feedback until the final treatment session; and the Task Only group performed the communicative tasks without receiving
any feedback. Treatment effects were measured through a grammaticality judgement test and an elicited imitation test. Working
memory was measured by means of an operation span test. The results revealed that working memory was a significant predictor only
of the effects of delayed CF, not those of immediate CF or task only. The findings suggest that delayed CF may have imposed a
heavier processing burden on the learners’ working memory due to the need to match the delayed feedback with the errors in their
procedural knowledge manifested in previous sessions. Based on the results of this and other empirical studies, the authors argue
for the superiority of immediate feedback over delayed feedback.
Anderson, D., Magill, R., & Sekiya, H. (2001). Motor learning as a function of KR schedule and characteristics of task-intrinsic feedback. Journal of Motor Behavior, 33(1), 59–66.
Anderson, D., Magill, R., Sekiya, H., & Ryan, G. (2005). Support for an explanation of the guidance effect in motor skill learning. Journal of Motor Behavior, 37(3), 231–238.
Arroyo, D. C., & Yilmaz, Y. (2018). An open for replication study: The role of feedback timing in synchronous computer-mediated
communication. Language Learning, 68(4), 942–972.
Baddeley, A. (2000). The episodic buffer: A new component of working memory?Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 4(11), 417–423.
Baddeley, A., & Hitch, G. (1974). Working memory. In G. Bower (Ed.), The Psychology of Learning and Motivation: Advances in Research and Theory (Vol. 81, pp. 47–89). New York, NY: Academic Press.
Case, R., Kurland, M., & Goldberg, J. (1982). Operational efficiency and the growth of short-term memory span. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 331, 386–404.
Conlin, J., Gathercole, S., & Adams, J. (2005). Children’s working memory: Investigating performance limitations in complex span tasks. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 90(4), 303–317.
Conway, A., Kane, M., Bunting, M., Hambrick, D., Wilhelm, O., & Engle, R. (2005). Working memory span tasks: A methodological review and user’s guide. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 12(5), 769–786.
DeKeyser, R. (2007). Practice in a second language: Perspectives from applied linguistics and cognitive psychology. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.
DeKeyser, R. (2012). Interactions between individual differences, treatments, and structures in SLA. Language Learning, 62(Suppl. 2), 189–200.
Doughty, C., & Varela, E. (1998). Communicative focus on form. In C. Doughty & J. Williams (Eds.), Focus on form in classroom second language acquisition (pp. 114–138). Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.
Ellis, R. (2005). Measuring implicit and explicit knowledge of a second language: A psychometric study. Studies in Second Language Acquisition, 27(02), 141–172.
Ellis, R. (2007). The differential effects of corrective feedback on two grammatical structures. In A. Mackey (Ed.), Conversational interaction in second language acquisition: A collection of empirical studies (pp. 339–360). Oxford: Oxford University Press.
Erlam, R., & Loewen, S. (2010). Implicit and explicit recasts in L2 oral French interaction. The Canadian Modern Language Review, 66(6), 887–916.
Gathercole, S., Pickering, S., Knight, C., & Stegmann, Z. (2004). Working memory skills and educational attainment: Evidence from national curriculum assessments at 7 and 14 years
of age. Applied Cognitive Psychology, 18(1), 1–16.
Goo, J. (2012). Corrective feedback and working memory capacity in interaction-driven L2 learning. Studies in Second Language Acquisition, 34(3), 445–474.
Kim, J., & Han, Z. (2007). Recasts in communicative EFL classes: Do teacher intent and learner interpretations overlap? In A. Mackey (Ed.), Conversational interaction in second language acquisition: A collection of empirical studies (pp. 269–297). Oxford: Oxford University Press.
Kim, Y., Payant, C., & Pearson, P. (2015). The intersection of task-based interaction, task complexity, and working memory. Studies in Second Language Acquisition, 371, 549–581.
Li, C., & Thompson, S. (1981). A functional reference grammar of Mandarin Chinese. Berkeley, CA: University of California Press.
Li, S. (2013a). The interactions between the effects of implicit and explicit feedback and individual differences in language
analytic ability and working memory. Modern Language Journal, 971, 634–654.
Li, S. (2013b). The differential roles of language analytic ability and working memory in mediating the effects of two types of
feedback on the acquisition of an opaque linguistic structure. In C. Sanz & B. Lado (Eds.), Individual differences, L2 development & language program administration: From theory to application (pp. 32–52). Boston, MA: Cengage Learning.
Li, S., Zhu, Y., & Ellis, R. (2016). The effects of the timing of corrective feedback on the acquisition of a new linguistic structure. The Modern Language Journal, 100(1), 276–295.
Li, S., & Fu, M. (2018). Strategic and unpressured within-task planning and their associations with working memory. Language Teaching Research, 22(2), 230–253.
Li, S., Ellis, R., & Zhu, Y. (2019). The associations between cognitive ability and L2 development under five different instructional
conditions. Applied Psycholinguistics, 1–30.
Linck, J., Osthus, P., Koeth, J., & Bunting, M. (2014). Working memory and second language comprehension and production: A meta-analysis. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 211, 861–883.
Mackey, A., & Sachs, R. (2012). Older learners in SLA research: A first look at working memory, feedback, and L2 development. Language Learning, 621, 704–740.
Mifka-Profozic, N. (2013). The effectiveness of corrective feedback and the role of individual differences in language learning. Frankfurt: Peter Lang.
Quinn, P. (2014). Delayed Versus Immediate Corrective Feedback on Orally Produced Passive Errors in English (Unpublished PhD dissertation). University of Toronto, Canada.
Révész, A. (2012). Working memory and the observed effectiveness of recasts on different L2 outcome measures. Language Learning, 621, 93–132.
Roche, K., & Chainay, H. (2013). Visually guided grasping of common objects: Effects of priming. Visual Cognition, 21(8), 1010–1032.
Salmoni, A., Schmidt, R., & Walter, C. (1984). Knowledge of results and motor learning: A review and critical reappraisal. Psychological Bulletin, 95(3), 355–386.
Sanz, C., Lin, H., Lado, B., Stafford, C., & Bowden, H. (2014). One size fits all? Learning conditions and working memory capacity in ab initio language
development. Applied Linguistics, 37(5), 669–692.
Schmidt, R., Young, D., Swinnen, S., & Shapiro, D. (1989). Summary knowledge of results for skill acquisition: Support for the guidance hypothesis. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 15(2), 352–359.
Schooler, L., & Anderson, J. (2008). The disruptive potential of immediate feedback. Retrieved from <[URL]>
Skinner, B. (1954). The science of learning and the art of teaching. Harvard Educational Review, 24(2), 86–97.
Spada, N., Shiu, J., & Tomita, Y. (2015). Validating an elicited imitation task as a measure of implicit knowledge: Comparisons with other validation
studies. Language Learning, 65(3), 723–751.
Wells, E., Kofler, M., Soto, E., Schaefer, H., & Sarver, D. (2018). Assessing working memory in children with ADHD: Minor administration and scoring changes may improve digit span
backward’s construct validity. Research in Developmental Disabilities, 721, 166–178.
Yang, Y., & Lyster, R. (2010). Effects of form-focused practice and feedback on Chinese EFL learners’ acquisition of regular and irregular past
tense forms. Studies in Second Language Acquisition, 32(02), 235–263.
Yilmaz, Y. (2013). Relative effects of explicit and implicit feedback: The role of working memory and language analytic
ability. Applied Linguistics, 341, 344–368.
Zhao, Y. (2015). The effects of explicit and implicit recasts on the acquisition of two grammatical structures and the mediating
role of working memory (Unpublished PhD dissertation). The University of Auckland, New Zealand.
Cited by (7)
Cited by seven other publications
Lee, Andrew H.
2024. The effects of proactive form-focused instruction and individual differences on second language acquisition. Language Teaching Research 28:6 ► pp. 2354 ff.
Rassaei, Ehsan
2024.
Immediate
vs.
delayed prompts, individual differences in working memory, and L2 development
. Language Awareness 33:3 ► pp. 649 ff.
Xu, Mingfei & Simin Zeng
2023. Optimal timing of treatment for errors in second language learning – A systematic review of corrective feedback timing. Frontiers in Psychology 14
2022. Corrective feedback, individual differences in working memory, and L2 development. Frontiers in Psychology 13
Mohammad Javad Ahmadian & Michael H. Long
2021. The Cambridge Handbook of Task-Based Language Teaching,
Révész, Andrea
2021. Methodological Approaches to Investigating Task-Based Language Teaching. In The Cambridge Handbook of Task-Based Language Teaching, ► pp. 605 ff.
This list is based on CrossRef data as of 10 january 2025. Please note that it may not be complete. Sources presented here have been supplied by the respective publishers.
Any errors therein should be reported to them.