The interaction between timing of explicit grammar explanation and individual differences in second language
acquisition
Despite numerous positive findings of explicit instruction, this topic continues to engage scholars worldwide. One
issue that may be crucial for the effectiveness of explicit instruction is the interaction between cognitive individual
differences (language aptitude and working memory) and types of instruction. In this experiment, 128 learners of Spanish were
randomly assigned to four experimental treatments and completed comprehension-based practice for interpreting object-verb and
ser/estar sentences in Spanish. Results revealed that the various combinations of rules and practice posed differential task
demands on the learners and consequently drew on language aptitude and working memory to a different extent. We argue that not
only are rules and practice both necessary, but that their suitable integration ameliorates task demands, reducing the burden on
the learner, and accordingly mitigates the role of participants’ individual differences, thus making a substantial difference for
the learning of second language grammar.
Article outline
- 1.Introduction
- 2.Background literature and theoretical motivation
- 2.1Language learning aptitude in the narrow sense
- 2.2Working memory
- 2.3Skill Acquisition Theory
- 3.The present study
- 3.1Method
- 3.1.1Participants
- 3.1.2Linguistic targets: OV structure and Ser/ Estar
- 3.1.3Research design
- 3.1.4Materials
- 3.1.5Treatment
- 3.2Instruments
- 3.2.1Comprehension tests
- 3.2.2Production test
- 3.2.3LLAMA F
- 3.2.4OSPAN
- 3.3Procedure
- 3.4Data analysis (Tables in Appendix)
- 4.Results
- 4.1OV
- 4.1.1Comprehension
- 4.1.2Production
- 4.1.3GJT
- 4.2SER
- 4.2.1Comprehension
- 4.2.2Production
- 4.2.3GJT
- 4.3Summary of results
- 4.4Simple linear regression models following significant correlations between IDs and outcome measures
- 5.Discussion and conclusions
- 6.Limitations and future directions
- 7.Conclusion
- Notes
-
References
References (99)
References
Ackerman, P. L. (1990). A correlational analysis of skill specificity: Learning, abilities, and individual differences. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 16(5), 883.
Ackerman, P. L. (1988). Determinants of individual differences during skill acquisition: Cognitive abilities and information processing. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 117(3), 288.
Anderson, J. R. (1993). Rules of the mind. London: Routledge.
Anderson, J. R. (1982). Acquisition of cognitive skill. Psychological Review, 89(4), 369.
Anderson, J. R., & Fincham, J. M. (1994). Acquisition of procedural skills from examples. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 20(6), 1322.
Anderson, J. R., Fincham, J. M., & Douglass, S. (1997). The role of examples and rules in the acquisition of a cognitive skill. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 23(4), 932.
Baddeley, A. D. (1986). Working memory. Oxford: Oxford University Press.
Baddeley, A. (2010). Working memory. Current Biology, 20(4), R136–R140.
Bates, E., MacWhinney, B., Caselli, C., Devescovi, A., Natale, F., & Venza, V. (1984). A cross-linguistic study of the development of sentence interpretation strategies. Child Development, 551, 341–354.
Benati, A. (2004). The effects of processing instruction, traditional instruction and meaning – output instruction on the acquisition of the English past simple tense. Language Teaching Research, 9(1), 67–93.
Bever, T. G. (1970). The cognitive basis for linguistic structures. In J. R. Hayes, (Ed.) Cognition and the development of language (pp. 279–362). New York, NY: Wiley.
Biedroń, A., & Pawlak, M. (2016). The interface between research on individual difference variables and teaching practice: The case of cognitive factors and personality. Studies in Second Language Learning and Teaching, 6(3), 395–422.
Brooks, P. J., Kempe, V., & Sionov, A. (2006). The role of learner and input variables in learning inflectional morphology. Applied Psycholinguistics, 27(2), 185–209.
Carroll, J. B. (1981). Language aptitude measures in streaming, matching students with methods, and diagnosis of learning problems. In K. C. Diller (Ed.), Individual differences and universals in language learning aptitude (pp. 83–118). Rowley, MA: Newbury House.
Daily, L. Z., Lovett, M. C., & Reder, L. M. (1998). Cross-task prediction of working memory performance: Working memory capacity as source activation. In Sixth Annual ACT-R Workshop, Fairfax, VA.
De Graaff, R. (1997). The eXperanto experiment. Studies in Second Language Acquisition, 19(02), 249–276.
De Jong, N., & Perfetti, C. A. (2011). Fluency training in the ESL classroom: An experimental study of fluency development and proceduralization. Language Learning, 61(2), 533–568.
DeKeyser, R. M. (1997). Beyond explicit rule learning. Studies in Second Language Acquisition, 19(02), 195–221.
DeKeyser, R. M. (1998). Beyond focus on form: Cognitive perspectives on learning and practic- ing second language grammar. In C. D. J. Williams (Ed.), Focus on form in classroom second language acquisition (pp. 42–63). Cambridge, England: Cambridge University Press.
DeKeyser, R. M. (2007). (Ed.). Practice in a second language: Perspectives from applied linguistics and cognitive psychology. New York, NY: Cambridge University Press.
DeKeyser, R. M. (2012). Interactions between individual differences, treatments, and structures in SLA. Language Learning, 62(s2), 189–200.
DeKeyser, R. (2013). Age effects in second language learning: Stepping stones toward better understanding. Language Learning, 63(Suppl. 1), 52–67.
DeKeyser, R. M. (2015). Skill Acquisition Theory. In B. VanPatten & J. Williams (Eds.), Theories in second language acquisition: An introduction (pp. 94–113). New York, NY: Routledge.
DeKeyser, R. M., & Koeth, J. (2011). Cognitive aptitudes for second language learning. In E. Hinkel (Ed.), Handbook of research in second language teaching and learning (Vol. 21, pp. 395–406). New York, NY: Routledge.
DeKeyser, R. M., & Criado-Sánchez, R. (2012). Automatization, skill acquisition, and practice in second language acquisition. In C. A. Chapelle (Ed.), The encyclopedia of applied linguistics (pp. 323–331). Oxford: Wiley-Blackwell.
Doughty, C., Campbell, S., Bunting, M., Bowles, A., & Haarmann, H. (2007). The development of the high-level language aptitude battery. Center for Advanced Study of Language Technical Report (TTO 2105 M. 4). College Park, MD: University of Maryland.
Engle, R. W. (2002). Working memory capacity as executive attention. Current directions in Psychological Science, 11(1), 19–23.
Erlam, R. (2003). The effects of deductive and inductive instruction on the acquisition of direct object pronouns in French as a second language. The Modern Language Journal, 87(2), 242–260.
Erlam, R. (2005). Language aptitude and its relationship to instructional effectiveness in second language acquisition. Language Teaching Research, 9(2), 147–171.
Farley, A. (2004a). The relative effects of processing instruction and meaning-based output instruction. In B. VanPatten (Eds.), Processing instruction: Theory, research, and commentary (pp. 143–168). Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.
Farley, A. (2004b). Processing instruction and the Spanish subjunctive: Is explicit information needed? In B. VanPatten (Ed.), Processing instruction: Theory, research, and commentary (pp. 227–239). Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.
Fernández, C. (2008). Reexamining the role of explicit information in processing instruction. Studies in Second Language Acquisition, 301, 277–305.
Fitts, P. M. (1964). Perceptual-motor skill learning. Categories of Human Learning, 471, 381–391.
French, L. M. & O’Brien, I. (2008). Phonological memory and children’s second language grammar learning. Applied Psycholinguistics, 291, 463–87.
Gass, S. M. (1989). Language universals and second-language acquisition. Language Learning, 39(4), 497–534.
Goo, J. (2012). Corrective feedback and working memory capacity in interaction-driven L2 learning. Studies in Second Language Acquisition, 341, 445–474.
Harrington, M., & Sawyer, M. (1992). L2 working memory capacity and L2 reading skill. Studies in Second Language Acquisition, 14(1), 25–38.
Henry, N., Culman, H., & Vanpatten, B. (2009). More on the effects of explicit information in instructed SLA . A partial replication and a response to Fernández (2008). Studies in Second Language Acquisition, 311, 559–575.
Huitema, B. (2011). The analysis of covariance and alternatives. New York, NY: John Wiley & Sons.
Juffs, A. (2004). Representation, processing and working memory in a second language. Transactions of the Philological Society, 1021, 199–226.
Juffs, A., & Harrington, M. W. (2011). Aspects of working memory in L2 Learning. Language Teaching: Reviews and Studies, 42(2), 137–166.
Just, M. A., & Carpenter, P. A. (1992). A capacity theory of comprehension: Individual differences in working memory. Psychological Review, 991, 122–149.
Kirschner, P. A., Sweller, J., & Clark, R. E. (2006). Why minimal guidance during instruction does not work: An analysis of the failure of constructivist, discovery, problem-based, experiential, and inquiry-based teaching. Educational Psychologist, 41(2), 75–86.
Kormos, J., & Sáfár, A. (2008). Phonological short-term memory, working memory and foreign language performance in intensive language learning. Bilingualism: Language and Cognition, 11(2), 261–271.
Lado, B., Bowden, H. W., Stafford, C. A., & Sanz, C. (2014). A fine-grained analysis of the effects of negative evidence with and without metalinguistic information in language development. Language Teaching Research, 18(3), 320–344.
Li, S. (2013). The interactions between the effects of implicit and explicit feedback and individual differences in language analytic ability and working memory. The Modern Language Journal, 97(3), 634–654.
Linck, J. A., Hughes, M. M., Campbell, S. G., Silbert, N. H., Tare, M., Jackson, S. R., … Doughty, C. J. (2013). Hi-LAB: A new measure of aptitude for high-level language proficiency. Language Learning, 63(3), 530–566.
Linck, J. A., Osthus, P., Koeth, J. T., & Bunting, M. F. (2014). Working memory and second language comprehension and production: A meta-analysis. Psychonomic bulletin & review, 21(4), 861–883.
Lovett, M. C., Reder, L. M., & Lebiere, C. (1997). Modeling individual differences in a digit working memory task. In Proceedings of the Nineteenth Annual Conference of the Cognitive Science Society (pp. 460–465).
Lyster, R. (1994). The effect of functional-analytic teaching on aspects of French immersion students’ sociolinguistic competence. Applied Linguistics, 15(3), 263–287.
Lyster, R. (2004). Research on form-focused instruction in immersion classrooms: Implications for theory and practice. Journal of French Language Studies, 141, 321–341.
Mackey, A., Philp, J., Egi, T., Fujii, A. & Tatsumi, T. (2002). Individual differences in working memory, noticing of interactional feedback, and L2 development. In P. Robinson (Ed.), Individual differences and instructed language learning (pp. 181–209). Amsterdam: John Benjamins.
Mackey, A., Adams, R., Stafford, C., & Winke, P. (2010). Exploring the relationship between modified output and working memory capacity. Language Learning, 60(3), 501–533.
MacWhinney, B. (2001). The competition model: The input, the context, and the brain. In P. Robinson (Ed.), Cognition and second language instruction (pp. 69–90). Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.
Martin, K. I., & Ellis, N. C. (2012). The roles of phonological STM and working memory in L2 grammar and vocabulary learning. Studies in Second Language Acquisition, 34(3), 379–413.
McLaughlin, B. (1990). Restructuring. Applied Linguistics, 11(2), 113–128.
Morgan-Short, K., Faretta-Stutenberg, M., Brill-Schuetz, K. A., Carpenter, H., & Wong, P. (2014). Declarative and procedural memory as individual differences in second language acquisition. Bilingualism: Language and Cognition, 17(1), 56–72.
Norris, J. M., & Ortega, L. (2000). Effectiveness of L2 instruction: A research synthesis and quantitative meta‐analysis. Language Learning, 50(3), 417–528.
O’Brien, I., Segalowitz, N., Collentine, J., & Freed, B. (2006). Phonological memory and lexical, narrative, and grammatical skills in second language oral production by adult learners. Applied Psycholinguistics, 271, 377–402.
Papagno, C., Valentine, T., & Baddeley, A. D. (1991). Phonological short-term memory and foreign-language vocabulary learning. Journal of Memory and Language, 301, 331–347.
Perrachione, T. K., Lee, J., Ha, L. Y., & Wong, P. C. (2011). Learning a novel phonological contrast depends on interactions between individual differences and training paradigm design. The Journal of the Acoustical Society of America, 130(1), 461–472.
Presson, N., MacWhinney, B., & Tokowicz, N. (2014). Learning grammatical gender: The use of rules by novice learners. Applied Psycholinguistics, 35(4), 709–737.
Prieto Botana, G. (2013). The role of task-essentialness and explicit information in processing instruction (Unpublished PhD dissertation). University of Maryland, College Park.
Ranta, L., & Lyster, R. (2007). A cognitive approach to improving immersion students’ oral production abilities: The awareness, practice, and feedback sequence. In R. DeKeyser (Ed.), Practicing in a second language: Perspectives from applied linguistics and cognitive psychology. New York, NY: Cambridge University Press.
Rehling, J., Demiral, B., Lebiere, C., Lovett, M., & Reder, L. M. (2003). Modeling individual difference factors in a complex task environment. In F. Detje, D. Doerner, & H. Schaub (Eds.), In Proceedings of the Fifth International Conference on Cognitive Modeling (pp. 287–288). Bamberg: Universitats-Verlag.
Révész, A. (2012). Working memory and the observed effectiveness of recasts on different L2 outcome measures. Language Learning, 62(1), 93–132.
Robinson, P. (1996). Learning simple and complex second language rules under implicit, incidental, rule-search, and instructed conditions. Studies in Second Language Acquisition, 18(1), 27–67.
Robinson, P. (1997). Generalizability and automaticity of second language learning under implicit, incidental, enhanced, and instructed conditions. Studies in Second Language Acquisition, 19(2), 223–247.
Robinson, P. (2005). Aptitude and second language acquisition. Annual Review of Applied Linguistics, 251, 46–73.
Robinson, P. (2007). Aptitudes, abilities, contexts, and practice. Practice in a second language: Perspectives from applied linguistics and cognitive psychology, 256–286.
Sallas, B., Mathews, R. C., Lane, S. M., & Sun, R. (2007). Developing rich and quickly accessed knowledge of an artificial grammar. Memory & Cognition, 35(8), 2118–2133.
Sanz, C., & Morgan-Short, K. (2004). Positive evidence versus explicit rule presentation and explicit negative feedback: A computer-assisted study. Language Learning, 541, 35–78.
Sanz, C., Lin, H. J., Lado, B., Stafford, C. A., & Bowden, H. W. (2014). One size fits all? Learning conditions and working memory capacity in ab initio language development. Applied Linguistics, 37(5), 669–692.
Serafini, E. J., & Sanz, C. (2016). Evidence for the decreasing impact of cognitive ability on second language development as proficiency increases. Studies in Second Language Acquisition, 381, 607–646.
Sheen, Y. (2007). The effect of focused written corrective feedback and language aptitude on ESL learners’ acquisition of articles. TESOL Quarterly, 41(2), 255–283.
Skehan, P. (1998). A cognitive approach to learning language. Oxford: Oxford University Press.
Skehan, P. (2012). Language aptitude. In S. M. Gass & A. Mackey (Eds.). The Routledge handbook of second language acquisition (pp. 381–395). New York, NY: Routledge.
Skehan, P. (2015a). Working memory and second language performance: A commentary. In Z. Wen, M. Borges Mota, & A. McNeill (Eds.), Working memory in second language acquisition and processing (pp. 189–202). Bristol: Multilingual Matters.
Skehan, P. (2015b). Foreign language aptitude and its relationship with grammar: A critical overview. Applied Linguistics, 361, 367–384.
Snow, R. E. (1989). Aptitude-treatment interaction as a framework for research on individual differences in learning. In P. L. Ackerman, R. J. Sternberg, & R. Glaser (Eds.), A series of books in psychology. Learning and individual differences: Advances in theory and research (pp. 13–59). New York, NY: W. H. Freeman/Times Books/ Henry Holt & Co.
Spada, N., & Tomita, Y. (2010). Interactions between type of instruction and yype of language reature: A meta‐analysis. Language Learning, 60(2), 263–308.
Stafford, C. A., Bowden, H. W., & Sanz, C. (2012). Optimizing language instruction: Matters of explicitness, practice, and cue learning. Language Learning, 62(3), 741–768.
Suzuki, Y., & DeKeyser, R. (2017). The interface of explicit and implicit knowledge in a second language: Insights from individual differences in cognitive aptitude. Language Learning, 671, 747–790.
Taatgen, N. A. (2001). A model of individual differences in learning air traffic control. In E. M. Altmann, A. Cleeremans, C. D. Schunn, & W. D. Gray (Eds.), Proceedings of the 2001 fourth International Conference on Cognitive Modeling (pp. 211–216). Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.
Taatgen, N. A., & Wallach, D. (2002). Whether skill acquisition is rule or instance based is determined by the structure of the task. Cognitive Science Quarterly, 2(2), 163–204.
Tagarelli, K. M., & Borges, M. (2015). Working memory, learning conditions and the acquisition of L2 syntax. In Z. Wen, M. Borges Mota, & A. McNeill (Eds.), Working memory in second language acquisition and processing: Theory, research and commentary (pp. 224–247). Bristol: Multilingual Matters.
VanPatten, B. (1987). Classroom learners’ acquisition of ser and estar: Accounting for developmental patterns. In B. VanPatten, T. R. Dvorak, & J. F. Lee (Eds.), Foreign language learning: A research perspective (pp. 61–75). Rowley, MA: Newbury House.
VanPatten, B., & Oikkenon, S. (1996). Explanation vs. structured input in processing instruction. Studies in Second Language Acquisition, 181, 495–510.
VanLehn, K. (1996). Cognitive skill acquisition. In J. Spence, J. Darly, & D. J. Foss (Eds.), Annual review of psychology (Vol. 471, pp. 513–539). Palo Alto, CA: Annual Reviews.
Wesche, M. (1981). Language aptitude measures in streaming, matching students with methods, and diagnosis of learning problems. In K. Diller (Ed.), Individual differences and universals in language learning aptitude (pp. 119–154). Rowley, MA: Newbury House.
Williams, J. N. (1999). Memory, attention, and inductive learning. Studies in Second Language Acquisition, 21(01), 1–48.
Wong, W. (2004). Processing instruction in French: The roles of explicit information and structured input. In B. VanPatten (Ed.), Processing instruction: Theory, research, and commentary (pp. 187–205). Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.
Cited by (1)
Cited by one other publication
Motlagh, Hossein Saadabadi, Halimeh Khafaie, Ali Asghar Arastoo, Maria Cheraghi & Morteza Abdullatif Khafaie
2020.
Application of social network in traditional sciences education on the vocabulary acquisition of secondary English learner students.
Education and Information Technologies 25:4
► pp. 3071 ff.
This list is based on CrossRef data as of 20 october 2024. Please note that it may not be complete. Sources presented here have been supplied by the respective publishers.
Any errors therein should be reported to them.