References
Baayen, R. H., Davidson, D. J., & Bates, D. M.
(2008) Mixed-effects modeling with crossed random effects for subjects and items. Journal of Memory and Language, 591, 390–412. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Beyersmann, E., Casalis, S., Ziegler, J. C., & Grainger, J.
(2014) Language proficiency and morpho-orthographic segmentation. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 22(4), 1054–1061. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Beyersmann, E., Cavalli, E., Casalis, S., & Colé, P.
(2016) Embedded stem priming effects in prefixed and suffixed pseudowords. Scientific Studies of Reading, 20(3), 220–230. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Beyersmann, E., & Grainger, J.
(2017) Support from the morphological family when unembedding the stem. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory and Cognition, 44(1), 135–142.Google Scholar
Ciaccio, L. A., & Clahsen, H.
(2020) Variability and consistency in first and second language processing: A masked morphological priming study on prefixation and suffixation. Language Learning, 701, 103–136. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Diependaele, K., Duñabeitia, J. A., Morris, J., & Keuleers, E.
(2011) Fast morphological effects in first and second language word recognition. Journal of Memory and Language, 641, 344–358. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Diependaele, K., Sandra, D., & Grainger, J.
(2009) Semantic transparency and masked morphological priming: The case of prefixed words. Memory & Cognition, 371, 895–908. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Finley, A., & Penningroth, S.
(2015) Online versus in-lab: Pros and cons of an online prospective memory experiment. In A. M. Columbus (Ed.), Advances in Psychology Research, vol. 113. pp. 135–162. Hauppauge, NY: Nova Science Publishers, Inc.Google Scholar
Fiorentino, R., & Fund-Reznicek, E.
(2009) Masked morphological priming of compound constituents. The Mental Lexicon, 41, 159–193. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Foote, R., Qasem, M., & Trentman, E.
(2020) Morphological decomposition in L2 Arabic: A masked priming study. Journal of Psycholinguistic Research, 491, 291–317. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Forster, K. I., & Davis, C.
(1984) Repetition priming and frequency attenuation in lexical access. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 101, 680–698.Google Scholar
Forster, K. I., & Veres, C.
(1998) The prime lexicality effect: Form-priming as a function of prime awareness, lexical status, and discrimination difficulty. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 241, 498–514.Google Scholar
Grainger, J., & Beyersmann, E.
(2017) Edge-aligned embedded word activation initiates morpho-orthographic segmentation. In Psychology of Learning and Motivation (Vol. 671, pp. 285–317). Academic Press.Google Scholar
Hasenäcker, J., Beyersmann, E., & Schroeder, S.
(2016) Masked morphological priming in German-speaking adults and children: Evidence from response time distributions. Frontiers in Psychology, 71, 929. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Heyer, V., & Clahsen, H.
(2015) Late bilinguals see a scan in scanner AND in scandal: Dissecting formal overlap from morphological priming in the processing of derived words. Bilingualism: Language and Cognition, 181, 543–550. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Kim, S. Y., Wang, M., & Taft, M.
(2015) Morphological decomposition in the recognition of prefixed and suffixed words: Evidence from Korean. Scientific Studies of Reading, 191, 183–203. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Kuznetsova, A., Brockhoff, P., & Christensen, R.
(2014) LmerTest: Tests for random and fixed effects for linear mixed effect models. R package, version 2.0-3.Google Scholar
Li, J. & Taft, M.
(2019) The processing of English prefixed words by Chinese-English bilinguals. Studies in Second Language Acquisition, 1–11.Google Scholar
Li, J., Taft, M., & Xu, J.
(2017) The processing of English derived words by Chinese-English bilinguals. Language Learning, 671, 858–884. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Li, M., Jiang, N., & Gor, K.
(2017) L1 and L2 processing of compound words: Evidence from masked priming experiments in English. Bilingualism: Language and Cognition, 201, 384–402. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Morris, J., Porter, J. H., Grainger, J., & Holcomb, P. J.
(2011) Effects of lexical status and morphological complexity in masked priming: An ERP study. Language and Cognitive Processes, 26(4–6), 558–599. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
New, B., Brysbaert, M., Veronis, J., & Pallier, C.
(2007) The use of film subtitles to estimate word frequencies. Applied Psycholinguistics, 28(4), 661. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Qiao, X., & Forster, K. I.
(2013) Novel word lexicalization and the prime lexicality effect. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 391, 1064–1074.Google Scholar
(2017) Is the L2 lexicon different from the L1 lexicon? Evidence from novel word lexicalization. Cognition, 1581, 147–152. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Rastle, K., & Davis, M. H.
(2008) Morphological decomposition based on the analysis of orthography. Language and Cognitive Processes, 23(7–8), 942–971. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Silva, R., & Clahsen, H.
(2008) Morphologically complex words in L1 and L2 processing: Evidence from masked priming experiments in English. Bilingualism: Language and Cognition, 111, 245–260. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Taft, M.
(1987) Morphographic processing. The BOSS re-emerges. In M. Coltheart (Ed.), Attention and performance, XII1. pp. 265–279. London: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates Limited.Google Scholar
(1991) Reading and the mental lexicon. Hove, UK: Erlbaum.Google Scholar
(2015) The nature of lexical representation in visual word recognition. In A. Pollatsek, & R. Treiman (Eds.) Handbook on Reading. pp. 99–113. New York: Oxford University Press. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Taft, M., & Forster, K. I.
(1975) Lexical storage and retrieval of prefixed words. Journal of Verbal Learning and Verbal Behavior, 141, 638–647. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Taft, M., & Li, J.
(2020) A new type of masked form priming: Native versus nonnative English speakers. Studies in Second Language Acquisition, 1–12. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Taft, M., Li, S., & Beyersmann, E.
(2018) What cross-morphemic letter transposition in derived nonwords tells us about lexical processing. Journal of Cognition, 11, 36. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Taft, M., Xu, J., & Li, S.
(2017) Letter coding in visual word recognition: The impact of embedded words. Journal of Memory and Language, 921, 14–25. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Taft, M., & Zhu, X.
(1997) Submorphemic processing in reading Chinese. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory and Cognition, 231, 761–775.Google Scholar