Article published In:
Studies of Bilingual Processing Presented to Kenneth I. Forster
Edited by Nan Jiang
[Journal of Second Language Studies 4:2] 2021
► pp. 375411
References
Adler, R. M., Valdés Kroff, J. R., & Novick, J. M.
(2020) Does integrating a code-switch during comprehension engage cognitive control? Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 46(4), 741.Google Scholar
Alnæs, D., Sneve, M. H., Espeseth, T., Endestad, T., van de Pavert, S. H. P., & Laeng, B.
(2014) Pupil size signals mental effort deployed during multiple object tracking and predicts brain activity in the dorsal attention network and the locus coeruleus. Journal of Vision, 14(1), 1–20. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Andrews, S.
(1992) Frequency and neighborhood effects on lexical access: Lexical similarity or orthographic redundancy? Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 18(2), 234–254.Google Scholar
Aston-Jones, G., & Cohen, J. D.
(2005) An integrative theory of locus coeruleus-norepinephrine function: adaptive gain and optimal performance. The Annual Review of Neuroscience, 281, 403–450. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Attar, N., Schneps, M., & Pomplun, M.
(2013) Pupil size as a measure of working memory load during a complex visual search task. Journal of Vision, 131, 160. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Backus, A.
(2012) A usage-based approach to borrowability. Tilburg Papers in Culture Studies, 271.Google Scholar
Beatty, J., & Kahneman, D.
(1996) Pupillary changes in two memory tasks. Psychonomic Science, 51, 371–372. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Beatty-Martínez, A. L., & Dussias, P. E.
(2017) Bilingual experience shapes language processing: Evidence from codeswitching. Journal of Memory and Language, 951, 173–189. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Blanco-Elorrieta, E., & Pylkkänen, L.
(2017) Bilingual language switching in the lab vs. in the wild: The spatio-temporal dynamics of adaptive language control. Journal of Neuroscience, 371, 9022–9036. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
(2018) Ecological Validity in Bilingualism Research and the Bilingual Advantage. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 22(12), 1117–1126. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Boada, R., Sánchez-Casas, R., Gavilán, J. M., Garcia-Albea, J. E., & Tokowicz, N.
(2013) Effect of multiple translations and cognate status on translation recognition performance of balanced bilinguals. Bilingualism: Language and Cognition, 16(1), 183–197. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Boersma, P., & Weenink, D.
(2020) Praat: doing phonetics by computer [Computer program]. Version 6.1.09, retrieved from [URL]
Bosma, E., Blom, E., Hoekstra, E., & Versloot, A.
(2019) A longitudinal study on the gradual cognate facilitation effect in bilingual children’s Frisian receptive vocabulary. International Journal of Bilingual Education and Bilingualism, 22(4), 371–385. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Bradshaw, J. L.
(1967) Pupil size as a measure of arousal during information processing. Nature, 2161, 515–516. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Byers-Heinlein, K., Morin-Lessard, E., & Lew-Williams, C.
(2017) Bilingual infants control their languages as they listen. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, 114(34), 9032–9037. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Carrol, S. E.
(1992) On cognates. Second Language Research, 8(2), 93–119.Google Scholar
Costa, A., Caramazza, A., & Sebastian-Gallés, N.
(2000) The cognate facilitation effect: implications for models of lexical access. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 26(5), 1283.Google Scholar
Davies, M.
(2016–) Corpus del Español: Two billion words, 21 countries. Available online at [URL]. (Web / Dialects)
Delong, K. A., Urbach, T. P., Groppe, D. M., & Kutas, M.
(2011) Overlapping dual ERP responses to low cloze probability sentence continuations. Psychophysiology, 48(9), 1203–1207. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Dijkstra, T., Grainger, J., & Van Heuven, W. J.
(1999) Recognition of cognates and interlingual homographs: The neglected role of phonology. Journal of Memory and language, 41(4), 496–518. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Dijkstra, T., Miwa, K., Brummelhuis, B., Sappelli, M., & Baayen, H.
(2010) How cross-language similarity and task demands affect cognate recognition. Journal of Memory and language, 62(3), 284–301. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Elston-Güttler, K. E., Gunter, T. C., & Kotz, S. A.
(2005) Zooming into L2: Global language context and adjustment affect processing of interlingual homographs in sentences. Cognitive Brain Research, 251, 57–70. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Fasiolo, M., Nedellec, R., Goude, Y., & Wood, S. N.
(2018) Scalable visualisation methods for modern Generalized Additive Models. Arxiv preprint. [URL]
Foote, S. L., & Morrison, J. H.
(1987) Extrathalamic modulation of cortical function. Annual Review of Neuroscience, 101, 67–95. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Fricke, M., Kroll, J. F., & Dussias, P. E.
(2016) Phonetic variation in bilingual speech: A lens for studying the production-comprehension link. Journal of Memory and Language, 891, 110–137. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Gabay, S., Pertzov, Y., & Henik, A.
(2011) Orienting of attention, pupil size, and the norepinephrine system. Attention, Perception, & Psychophysics, 731, 123–129. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Gagl, B., Hawelka, S., & Hutzler, F.
(2011) Systematic influence of gaze position on pupil size measurement: analysis and correction. Behavior Research Methods, 43(4), 1171–1181. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Goldrick, M., Putnam, M., & Schwartz, L.
(2016) Coactivation in bilingual grammars: A computational account of code mixing. Bilingualism: Language and Cognition, 19(5), 857–876. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Goldwater, B. C.
(1972) Psychological significance of pupillary movements. Psychological Bulletin, 771, 340–355. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Gollan, T. H., Slattery, T. J., Goldenberg, D., Van Assche, E., Duyck, W., & Rayner, K.
(2011) Frequency drives lexical access in reading but not in speaking: The frequency-lag hypothesis. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 140(2), 186. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Grosjean, F.
(1995) A psycholinguistic approach to code-switching: The recognition of guest words by bilinguals. One speaker, two languages: Cross-disciplinary perspectives on code-switching, 259–275. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Guasch, M., Ferré, P., & Haro, J.
(2017) Pupil dilation is sensitive to the cognate status of words: further evidence for non-selectivity in bilingual lexical access. Bilingualism: Language and Cognition, 20(1), 49–54. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Gunter, T. C., Friederici, A. D., & Schriefers, H.
(2006) Syntactic Gender and Semantic Expectancy: ERPs Reveal Early Autonomy and Late Interaction. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 12(4), 556–568. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Hartsuiker, R. J., Pickering, M. J., & Veltkamp, E.
(2004) Is Syntax Separate or Shared between Languages? Cross-Linguistic Syntactic Priming in Spanish-English Bilinguals. Psychological Science, 15(6), 409–414. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Haspelmath, M.
(2009) Lexical borrowing: Concepts and issues. In M. Haspelmath and U. Tadmor (Eds.), Loanwords in the World’s Languages (pp. 35–54). De Gruyter Mouton. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Hoshino, N., & Kroll, J. F.
(2008) Cognate effects in picture naming: Does cross-language activation survive a change of script? Cognition, 106(1), 501–511. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Jescheniak, J. D., & Schriefers, H.
(1998) Discrete serial versus cascaded processing in lexical access in speech production: Further evidence from the coactivation of near-synonyms. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 24(5), 1256–1274.Google Scholar
Johns, M. A., Valdés Kroff, J. R., & Dussias, P. E.
(2019) Mixing things up: How blocking and mixing affect the processing of codemixed sentences. International Journal of Bilingualism, 23(2), 584–611. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Kay, G.
(1995) English loanwords in Japanese. World Englishes, 14(1), 67–76. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Keuleers, E., & Brysbaert, M.
(2010) Wuggy: A multilingual pseudoword generator. Behavior Research Methods 42(3), 627–633. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Kuchinsky, S. E., Ahlstrom, J. B., Vaden Jr., K. I., Cute, S. L., Humes, L. E., Dubno, J. R., & Eckert, M. A.
(2013) Pupil size varies with word listening and response selection difficulty in older adults with hearing loss. Psychophysiology, 501, 23–34. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Kuitert, S. R.
(2013) English Loanwords in Norwegian: A Loanword Processing Study in Young Speakers of Norwegian (Master’s thesis, Norges teknisk-naturvitenskapelige universitet, Trondheim, Norway). Retrieved from: [URL]
Kutas, M. & Hillyard, S. A.
(1980a) Reading senseless sentences: brain potentials reflect semantic incongruity. Science, 207(4427), 203–205. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
(1980b) Event-related brain potentials to semantically inappropriate and surprisingly large words. Biological Psychology, 11(2), 99–116. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
(1984) Brain potentials during reading reflect word expectancy and semantic association. Nature, 3071, 161–163. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Lagrou, E., Hartsuiker, R. J., & Duyck, W.
(2011) Knowledge of a second language influences auditory word recognition in the native language. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 37(4), 952–965.Google Scholar
Libben, M. R., & Titone, D. A.
(2009) Bilingual lexical access in context: evidence from eye movements during reading. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 35(2), 381.Google Scholar
Linck, J. A., Kroll, J. F., & Sunderman, G.
(2009) Losing access to the native language while immersed in a second language: Evidence for the role of inhibition in second-language learning. Psychological Science, 20(12), 1507–1515. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Litcofsky, K. A., & Van Hell, J. G.
(2017) Switching direction affects switching costs: Behavioral, ERP, and time-frequency analyses of intra-sentential codeswitching. Neuropsychologia, 971, 112–139. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Makowski, D.
(2018) The Psycho Package: An Efficient and Publishing-Oriented Workflow for Psychological Science. Journal of Open Source Software, 3(22), 470. Available from [URL]. DOI logo
Morales, A.
(2009) Diccionario de anglicismos actuales: fotografía desde la Internet. Academia Puertorriqueña de la Lengua Española.Google Scholar
Moreno, E. M., Federmeier, K. D., & Kutas, M.
(2002) Switching Languages, Switching Palabras (Words): An Electrophysiological Study of Code Switching. Brain and Language, 801, 188–207. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Muysken, P.
(2013) Language contact outcomes as the result of bilingual optimization strategies. Bilingualism: Language and Cognition, 16(4), 709–730. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Myers-Scotton, C.
(1993) Duelling Languages. New York, NY: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
(2002) Contact linguistics: Bilingual encounters and grammatical outcomes. Oxford: Oxford University Press. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
(2006) Multiple voices: An introduction to bilingualism. Malden, MA: Blackwell.Google Scholar
Peeters, D., Dijkstra, T., & Grainger, J.
(2013) The representation and processing of identical cognates by late bilinguals: RT and ERP effects. Journal of Memory and Language, 68(4), 315–332. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Poplack, S.
(1980) Sometimes I’ll start a sentence in Spanish y termino en español. Linguistics, 18(7/8), 581–618. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
(2012) What does the Nonce Borrowing Hypothesis hypothesize? Bilingualism: Language and Cognition, 15(3), 644–648. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
(2018) Borrowing: Loanwords in the Speech Community and in the Grammar. New York, NY: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Poplack, S., Sankoff, M., & Miller, C.
(1988) The social correlates and linguistic processes of lexical borrowing and assimilation. Linguistics, 261, 47–104. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
R Core Team
(2021) R: A language and environment for statistical computing. R Foundation for Statistical Computing, Vienna, Austria. URL: [URL]
Samuels, E. R., & Szabadi, E.
(2008) Functional neuroanatomy of the noradrenergic locus coeruleus: its role in the regulation of arousal and autonomic function part I: principles of functional organisation. Current Neuropharmacology, 61, 235–253. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Sankoff, D., Poplack, S., & Vanniarajan, S.
(1990) The case of the nonce loan in Tamil. Language Variation and Change, 21, 71–101. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Schmid, H. J.
(2007) Entrenchment, salience, and basic levels. The Oxford Handbook of Cognitive Linguistics, 1171, 138.Google Scholar
Schmidtke, J.
(2014) Second language experience modulates word retrieval effort in bilinguals: evidence from pupillometry. Frontiers in Psychology, 51. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
(2018) Pupillometry in Linguistic Research: An introduction and review for second language researchers. Studies in Second Language Acquisition, 401, 529–549. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Schoonbaert, S., Hartsuiker, R. J., & Pickering, M. J.
(2007) The representation of lexical and syntactic information in bilinguals: Evidence from syntactic priming. Journal of Memory and Language, 56(2), 153–171. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Schuster, S., Himmelstoss, N. A., Hutzler, F., Richlan, F., Kronbichler, M., & Hawelka, S.
(2021) Cloze enough? Hemodynamic effects of predictive processing during natural reading. NeuroImage, 2281, 117–687. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Segui, J., Mehler, J., Frauenfelder, U., & Morton, J.
(1982) The word frequency effect and lexical access. Neuropsychologia, 20(6), 615–627. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Sirois, S., & Brisson, J.
(2014) Pupillometry. Wiley Interdisciplinary Reviews: Cognitive Science, 5(6), 679–692. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Stammers, J. R., & Deuchar, M.
(2012) Testing the nonce borrowing hypothesis: Counter-evidence from English-origin verbs in Welsh. Bilingualism: Language and Cognition, 15(3), 630–643. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Strijkers, K., Costa, A., & Thierry, G.
(2010) Tracking lexical access in speech production: electrophysiological correlates of word frequency and cognate effects. Cerebral Cortex, 20(4), 912–928. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Tamaoka, K. & Miyaoka, Y.
(2003) The cognitive processing of Japanese loanwords in katakana. Japanese Psychological Research, 45(2), 69–79. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Thierry, G., & Wu, Y. J.
(2007) Brain potentials reveal unconscious translation during foreign-language comprehension. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, 104(30), 12530–12535. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Thomason, S. G. & Kaufman, T.
(1988) Language contact, creolization, and genetic linguistics. Berkley, CA: University of California Press. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Titone, D., Libben, M., Mercier, J., Whitford, V., & Pivneva, I.
(2011) Bilingual lexical access during L1 sentence reading: The effects of L2 knowledge, semantic constraint, and L1–L2 intermixing. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 37(6), 1412–1431.Google Scholar
Torres Cacoullos, R., & Travis, C. E.
(2015) Gauging convergence on the ground: Code-switching in the community. International Journal of Bilingualism, 19(4), 365–386. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
(2018) Bilingualism in the Community: Codeswitching and grammars in contact. Cambridge University Press. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Travis, C. E., Torres Cacoullos, R., & Kidd, E.
(2017) Cross-language priming: A view from bilingual speech. Bilingualism: Language and Cognition, 20(2), 283–298. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Van Assche, E., Duyck, W., Hartsuiker, R. J., & Diependaele, K.
(2009) Does bilingualism change native-language reading? Cognate effects in a sentence context. Psychological science, 20(8), 923–927. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
van Hout, R. & Muysken, P.
(1994) Modeling lexical borrowability. Language Variation and Change, 61, 39–62. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
van Rij, J., Hendriks, P., Van Rijn, H., Harald Baayen, R., & Wood, S. N.
(2019) Analyzing the Time Course of Pupillometric Data. Trends in Hearing, 231, 1–22. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
van Rij, J., Wieling, M., Baayen, R. H., & Van Rijn, H.
(2017) itsadug: Interpreting time series and autocorrelated data using GAMMs. R package version 2.3Google Scholar
Wetzel, N., Buttelmann, D., Schieler, A., & Widmann, A.
(2016) Infant and adult pupil dilation in response to unexpected sounds. Developmental Psychobiology, 58(3), 382–392. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Wieling, M.
(2018) Analyzing dynamic phonetic data using generalized additive mixed modeling: A tutorial focusing on articulatory differences between L1 and L2 speakers of English. Journal of Phonetics, 701, 86–116. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Wood, S. N.
(2011) Fast stable restricted maximum likelihood and marginal likelihood estimation of semiparametric generalized linear models. Journal of the Royal Statistical Society: Series B (Statistical Methodology), 73(1), 3–36. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
(2017) Generalized additive models: an introduction with R. Chapman and Hall/CRC. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Wood, S. N., Pya, N., & Saefken, B.
(2016) Smoothing parameter and model selection for general smooth models. Journal of the American Statistical Association, 1111, 1548–1575. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Wood, S. N.
(2003) Thin-plate regression splines. Journal of the Royal Statistical Society (B), 65(1), 95–114. DOI logoGoogle Scholar