References (109)
References
Avrutin, S. & Wexler, K. 1992. Development of principle B in Russian: Coindexation at LF and coreference. Language Acquisition 2: 259-306. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Baauw, S. 1999. The role of the clitic-full pronoun distinction in the acquisition of pronominal coreference. In Proceedings of the 23rd Annual Boston University Conference on Language Development, A. Greenhill, H. Littlefield & C. Tano (eds), 32-43. Somerville MA: Cascadilla Press.Google Scholar
Baauw S., Escobar, M. & Philip, W. 1997. A delay of principle B effect in Spanish speaking children: The role of lexical feature acquisition. In Proceedings of the GALA 97 Conference on Language Acquisition, A. Sorace, C. Heycock & R. Shillcock (eds), 16 -21. Edinburgh: University of Edinburgh.Google Scholar
Bahlmann J., Schubotz R.I. & Friederici, A.D. 2008. Hierarchical artificial grammar processing engages Broca’s area. NeuroImage 42(2): 525-34. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Berwick, C.R. 2011. All you need is Merge. All you need is Merge: Biology, computation, and language from the bottom up. In The Biolinguistic Enterprise: New Perspectives on the Evolution and Nature of the Human Language Faculty, A.M. Di Sciullo & C. Boeckx (eds), 461–491. Oxford: OUP.Google Scholar
Berwick, R.C., Beckers, G.J., Okanoya, K. & Bolhuis, J.J. 2012. A bird’s eye view of human language evolution. Front. Evol. Neurosci. 4(5). DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Berwick C.R. & Chomsky, N. 2011. The Biolinguistic Program. The current state of its development. In The Biolinguistic Enterprise: New Perspectives on the Evolution and Nature of the Human Language Faculty, A.M. Di Sciullo & C. Boeckx (eds), 13–45. Oxford: OUP.Google Scholar
Bickerton, D. 1990. Language and Species. Chicago IL: University of Chicago Press.Google Scholar
Boeckx, C. 2011. The emergence of the language faculty, from a biolinguistic point of view. In The Oxford Handbook of Language Evolution, M. Tallerman & K. Gibson (eds), 492-201. Oxford: OUP.Google Scholar
Bolhuis, J.J. & Everaert, M. (eds). 2013. Birdsong, Speech, and Language: Exploring the Evolution of Mind and Brain. Cambridge MA: The MIT Press.Google Scholar
Byrne, R.W. & Russon, A.E. 1998. Learning by imitation: A hierarchical approach. Brain and Behavioral Sciences 21: 667-721. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Chien, Y.C. & Wexler, K. 1990. Children’s knowledge of locality conditions in bindings as evidence for the modularity of syntax and pragmatics. Language Acquisition 1: 225–295. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Chomsky, N. 2015. Problems of projections: Extensions. In Structures, strategies and beyond: Studies in honour of Adriana Belletti, E. Di Domenico, C. Hamann & S. Matteini (eds), 1–16. Amsterdam: John Benjamins. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
. 2013. Problems of projections. Lingua 130: 33–49. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
. 2011. Poverty of the stimulus unfinished business. Ms, MIT.Google Scholar
. 2008b.The Biolinguistic Program: Where does it stand today? Ms, MIT.Google Scholar
. 2008a. On phases. In Foundational Issues in Linguistic Theory. Essays in Honor of Jean-Roger Vergnaud, R. Freiden, C.P. Otero & M.L. Zubizarreta (eds), 133–166. Cambridge MA: The MIT Press. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
. 2007. Of mind and language. Biolinguistics 1: 9-27.Google Scholar
. 2005. Three factors in language design. Linguistic Inquiry 36: 1–22. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
. 2001. Derivation by phase. In Ken Hale: A Life in Language, M. Kenstowicz (ed.), 1–52. Cambridge MA: The MIT Press.Google Scholar
. 1995. The Minimalist Program. Cambridge MA: The MIT Press.Google Scholar
. 1988. Language and Problems of Knowledge. Cambridge MA: The MIT Press.Google Scholar
. 1986. Knowledge of Language: Its Nature, Origins and Use. New York NY: Praeger.Google Scholar
. 1981. Lectures on Government and Binding: The Pisa lectures. Berlin: Mouton de Gruyter.Google Scholar
. 1968. Language and Mind. New York NY: Harcourt, Brace & World. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
. 1965. Aspects of the Theory of Syntax. Cambridge MA: The MIT Press.Google Scholar
. 1957. Three models for the description of language. IRE Trans. Inf. Theory 2: 113–124. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Collins, C. 1997. Local Economy. Cambridge MA: The MIT Press.Google Scholar
Coolidge, F.L & Wynn, T. 2005. Working memory, its executive functions, and the emergence of modern thinking. Cambridge Archaeological Journal 15(1): 5-26. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Darwin, C. 1871. The Descent of Man, and Selection in Relation to Sex. London: John Murray. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Di Sciullo, A.M. 2014. Minimalism and I-morphology. In Minimalism and Beyond: Radicalizing the Interfaces [Linguistic Faculty and Beyond 11], P. Kosta, S. Franks & T. Radeva-Bork (eds), 267–286. Amsterdam: John Benjamins. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
. 2013. Exocentric compounds, language and proto-language. Language Information Society 20: 1–26.Google Scholar
. 2012. Biolinguistics, minimalist grammars and the emergence of complex numerals. Presented at Theoretical Linguistics/Biolinguistics Workshop (EVOLANG 9) , 13 March, Kyoto.
. 2011. A biolinguistic approach to variation. In The Biolinguistic Enterprise: New Perspectives on the Evolution and Nature of the Human Language Faculty. A.M. Di Sciullo & C. Boeckx (eds), 305-328. Oxford: OUP.Google Scholar
. 2004. Morphological phases. In The 4th GLOW in Asia 2003. Generative Grammar in a Broader Perspective, J.-Y. Yoon (ed.), 113-137. The Korean Generative Grammar Circle and Cognitive Science, Seoul National University.Google Scholar
. 2005. Asymmetry in Morphology. Cambridge MA: The MIT Press.Google Scholar
Di Sciullo, A.M. & Somesfalean, S. 2015. Object pronouns in the evolution of Romanian: a biolinguistic perspective. In Formal approaches to DPs in Old Romanian, V. Hill (ed.), 269-290. Leiden/Boston: Brill. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Di Sciullo, A.M., Nicolis, M. & Somesfalean, S. 2013. Evo-devo language universals. Paper presented at the International Linguists Conference 19 , University of Geneva.
Di Sciullo, A.M. & Somesfalean, S. 2013. Variation in the Position of the Definite Determiner in Romanian: a Biolinguistic Perspective. Romance Linguistics in the Pacific: Variations in Time and Space. Special Issue of the Australian Journal of Linguistics 33(2): 121-139. Taylor&Francis.Google Scholar
Di Sciullo, A.M. et al. 2010. The Biological Nature of Human Language. Biolinguistics 4: 4-34.Google Scholar
Di Sciullo, A.M. & Isac, D. 2008. The asymmetry of Merge. Biolinguistics 2: 260–290.Google Scholar
Di Sciullo, A.M. & Aguero-Bautista, C. 2008. The delay of principle B effect (DPBE) and its absence in some languages. Language and Speech 51:77-100. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Embick, D. 2010. Localism versus Globalism in Morphology and Phonology. Cambridge MA: The MIT Press. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Fitch, W.T. 2011. “Deep homology’’ in the biology and evolution of language. In The Biolinguistic Enterprise: New Perspectives on the Evolution and Nature of the Human Language Faculty, A.M. Di Sciullo & C. Boeckx (eds), 135–166. Oxford: OUP.Google Scholar
Fitch, W.T. & Hauser, M.D. 2004. Computational constraints on syntactic processing in a nonhuman primate. Science 303: 377-380. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Fox, D. 2000. Scope and Semantic Interpretation. Cambridge MA: The MIT Press.Google Scholar
Gallistel, C.R. 1990. The Organization of Learning. Cambridge MA: Bradford Books/The MIT Press.Google Scholar
Gopnik, M. 1990. Feature blindness: A case study. Language Acquisition 1(2): 139-164. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Grodzinsky, J. & Reinhart, T.1993. The innateness of binding and coreference. Linguistic Inquiry 24: 69-102.Google Scholar
Hall, B.K. Descent with modification: The unity underlying homology and homoplasy as seen through an analysis of development and evolution. Biological Reviews 78: 409–433. DOI logo
Hauser, M.D., Chomsky, N. & Fitch, T. 2002. The faculty of language: What is it, who has it, and how did it evolve. Science 198: 1569-79. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Hauser, M.D. 2000. What do animals think about numbers? American Scientist 88(2): 144-151. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Hauser, M.D., MacNeilage, P. & Ware, M. 1996. Numerical representations in primates. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America 93(4): 1514-1517. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Hauser, M.D., Yang, C., Berwick, R.C., Tattersall, I., Ryan, M.J., Watumull, J., Chomsky, N. & Lewontin, R.C. 2014. The mystery of language acquisition. Frontiers in Psychology 5. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Hornstein, N. & Uriagereka, J. 2002. Reprojections. In Derivation and explanation in the Minimalist Program, S. Epstein & T. Seely (eds), 106-132. Malden MA: Blackwell. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Huang, J. 1998[1982]. Logical Relations in Chinese and the Theory of Grammar. New York NY: Garland.Google Scholar
Hurford, J.R. 2014. Origins of Langage. A Slim Guide. Oxford: OUP.Google Scholar
Jackendoff, J.R. 2011. What is the human language faculty: Two views? Language 87(3): 586-624. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Jenkins, L. 2011. Biolinguistic investigations: Genetics and dynamics. In The Biolinguistic Enterprise: New Perspectives on the Evolution and Nature of the Human Language Faculty, A.M. Di Sciullo & C. Boeckx (eds), 126–134. Oxford: OUP.Google Scholar
. 2000. Biolinguistics: Exploring the Biology of Language. Cambridge: CUP. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Kayne, R. 2011. Why are there no directionality parameters? In Proceedings of the 28th West Coast Conference on Formal Linguistics, M. Byram Washburn, K. McKinneyBock, E. Varis, A. Sawyer & B. Tomaszewicz (eds), 1-23. Somerville MA: Cascadilla Press.Google Scholar
. 1994. The Antisymmetry of Syntax. Cambridge MA: The MIT Press.Google Scholar
Koster, C. 1993. Errors in Anaphora Acquisition. PhD dissertation, Utrecht University.Google Scholar
Larson, R. 2011. Clauses, propositions and phases. In The Biolinguistic Enterprise: New Perspectives on the Evolution and Nature of the Human Language Faculty, A.M. Di Sciullo & C. Boeckx (eds), 366–391. Oxford: OUP.Google Scholar
Lasnik, H. 2011. What kind of computing device is the human language faculty. In The Biolinguistic Enterprise: New Perspectives on the Evolution and Nature of the Human Language Faculty, A.M. Di Sciullo & C. Boeckx (eds), 354–365. Oxford: OUP.Google Scholar
Lenneberg, E.H. 1967. Biological Foundations of Language. New York NY: Wiley & Sons.Google Scholar
Levin, M. & Palmer A.R. 2007. Left-right patterning from the inside out: Widespread evidence for intracellular control. BioEssays 29: 271-287. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Lewontin R.C. 2000. The Triple Helix: Gene, Organism and Environment. Cambridge MA: Harvard University Press.Google Scholar
Lewis. T. 2013. Ants ‘use math’ to find fastest route. Livescience. <[URL]>Google Scholar
Lewontin R. 2004. Response to ‘Selection for Asymmetry”. Science 306: 812. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Li, R. & Bowerman, B. 2010. Symmetry breaking in biology. Cold Spring Harbor Perspectives in Biology 2(3): A003475. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Lightfoot, D. 1991. How to Set Parameters. Arguments from Language Change. Cambridge MA: The MIT Press.Google Scholar
. 1982. The Language Lottery. Toward a Biology of Grammars. Cambridge MA: The MIT Press.Google Scholar
. 1979. Principles of Diachronic Syntax. Cambridge: CUP.Google Scholar
Marantz, A. 2008. Phases and words. In Phases in the Theory of Grammar, S.-H. Choe (ed.), 191–222. Seoul: Dong In.Google Scholar
McKee, C. 1992, A comparison of pronouns and anaphors in Italian and English acquisition. Language Acquisition 2: 21–54. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
McGonigle, B., Chalmers M. & Dickinson, A. 2003. Concurrent disjoint and reciprocal classification by Cebus Apella in seriation tasks: Evidence for hierarchical organization. Animal Cognition 6:185-197. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Moro, A. 2000. Dynamic Antisymmetry. Cambridge MA: The MIT Press.Google Scholar
Musso, M., Moro, A., Glauche, V., Rijntjes, M., Reichenbach, J., Büchel, C. & Weiller, C.. 2003. Broca’s area and the language instinct. Nature Neuroscience 6(7): 774‑781. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Nobrega, V.A. & Miyagawa, S. 2015. The precedence of syntax in the rapid emergence of human language in evolution as defined by the Integration Hypothesis. Frontiers Psychology 6: 271.Google Scholar
Palmer, R. 2009. Animal asymmetry. Current Biology 19(12): 473-477. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Palmer, A.R. 2004. Symmetry breaking and the evolution of development. Science 306: 828-833. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
. 1996. From symmetry to asymmetry: Phylogenetic patterns of asymmetry variation in animals and their evolutionary significance. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences (USA) 93: 14279-14286.
Philip, W. & Coopmans, P. 1996. The role of lexical feature acquisition in the development of pronominal anaphora. In Amsterdam Series on Child Language Development 5, W. Philip & F. Wijnen (eds). Amsterdam: University of Amsterdam.Google Scholar
Piattelli-Palmarini, M. (ed.). 1980. Language and Learning: The Debate between Jean Piaget and Noam Chomsky. Cambridge MA: Harvard University Press.Google Scholar
Piattelli Palmarini, M. & Uriagereka, J. 2011. A geneticist’s dream, a linguist’s nightmare: The case of FOXP2. In The Biolinguistic Enterprise: New Perspectives on the Evolution and Nature of the Human Language Faculty, A.M. Di Sciullo & C. Boeckx (eds), 100–125. Oxford: OUP.Google Scholar
Piattelli-Palmarini, M. & Berwick, R.C. (eds). 2013. Rich Languages from Poor Inputs. Oxford: OUP.Google Scholar
Pinker, S. 1994. The Language Instinct: How the Mind Creates Language. New York NY: HarperCollins. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Pinker, S. & Bloom, P. 1990. Natural language and natural selection. Behavioral and Brain Sciences 13(4): 707-784. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Poeppel, D. & Embick, D. 2005. Defining the relation between linguistics and neuroscience. In Twenty-First Century Psycholinguistics: Four Cornerstones, A. Cutler (ed.), 103-118. Mahwah NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.Google Scholar
Progovac, L. 2015. Evolutionary Syntax. Oxford: OUP. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
. 2007. Grammars within without recursion: Implications for evolutionary studies. Paper presented to Illinois State University Conference on Recursion in Human Languages , Normal IL.
. 2006. The syntax of nonsententials: Small clauses and phrases at the root. In The Syntax of Nonsententials [Linguistik Aktuell/Linguistics Today 93], L. Progovac, K. Paesani, E. Casielles & E. Barton (eds), 33–71. Amsterdam: John Benjamins. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Richards, N. 2002. Very local A’ movement in a root-first derivation. In Derivation and Explanation in the Minimalist Program, S.D. Epstein & T.D. Seely, 227-245. Oxford: Blackwell. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
. 2006. Deriving the edge: What’s in a phase? Ms, University of Cambridge.Google Scholar
Rizzi, L. 1990. Relativized Minimality. Cambridge MA: The MIT Press.Google Scholar
Roberts, I. 2011. Parametric hierarchies: Some observations. Presented at the workshop on linguistic variation and the minimalist program , Universidad Autonoma de Madrid.
. 2007. Diachronic Syntax. Oxford: OUP.Google Scholar
Roberts, I. & Roussou, A. 2003. Syntactic Change: A Minimalist Approach to Grammaticalization. Cambridge: CUP. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Rugani, R., Vallortigara, G., Vallini, B. & Regolin, L. 2011. Asymmetrical number-space mapping in the avian brain. Neurobiology of Learning and Memory 95: 231‑238. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Rugani, R., Fontanari, L., Simoni, E., Regolin, L. & Vallortigara, G. 2009. Arithmetic in newborn chicks. Proceedings of the Royal Society of London B , Biological Sciences 276: 2451–2460. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Schino, G., Tiddi, B. & di Sorrento, P. 2006. Simultaneous classification by rank and kinship in Japanese macaques. Animal Behaviour 71: 1069-1074. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Seyfarth, R.M., Cheney, D.L. & Bergman, T.J. 2005. Primate social cognition and the origins of language. Trends in Cognitive Sciences 9(6): 264‑266. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Shubin, N., Tabin, C. & Carroll, S. 1997. Fossils, genes and the evolution of animal limbs. Nature 388: 639–648. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Uriagereka, J. 2008. Syntactic Anchors. On Semantic Structuring. Cambridge: CUP. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
. 2002. Derivations. London: Routledge.Google Scholar
. 1999. Multiple spell-out. In Working Minimalism, S.D. Epstein & N. Hornstein (eds), 251-282. Cambridge MA: The MIT Press.Google Scholar
. 1998. Rime and Reason. Cambridge MA: The MIT Press.Google Scholar
Wexler, K. 1998. Very early parameter setting and the unique checking constraint: A new explanation of the optional infinitive stage. Lingua 106: 23-79. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Cited by (1)

Cited by one other publication

Haspelmath, Martin
2020. Human Linguisticality and the Building Blocks of Languages. Frontiers in Psychology 10 DOI logo

This list is based on CrossRef data as of 25 july 2024. Please note that it may not be complete. Sources presented here have been supplied by the respective publishers. Any errors therein should be reported to them.