To be specified published in:
Aging and Bilingualism
Edited by Ellen Bialystok and Margot D. Sullivan
[Linguistic Approaches to Bilingualism 6:1/2] 2016
► pp. 119146
Cited by

Cited by 12 other publications

Blumenfeld, Henrike K., Sim J. R. Quinzon, Cindy Alsol & Stephanie A. Riera
2017. Predictors of Successful Learning in Multilingual Older Adults Acquiring a Majority Language. Frontiers in Communication 2 Crossref logo
Bobb, Susan C. & Judith F. Kroll
2018.  In Bilingual Cognition and Language [Studies in Bilingualism, 54],  pp. 307 ff. Crossref logo
Chabal, Sarah, Sayuri Hayakawa & Viorica Marian
2021. How a picture becomes a word: individual differences in the development of language-mediated visual search. Cognitive Research: Principles and Implications 6:1 Crossref logo
Donnelly, Seamus, Patricia J. Brooks & Bruce D. Homer
2019. Is there a bilingual advantage on interference-control tasks? A multiverse meta-analysis of global reaction time and interference cost. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review 26:4  pp. 1122 ff. Crossref logo
Ferreira, Geise Corrêa, Enma Mariángel Ortiz Torres, Michele Vargas Garcia, Sinéia Neujahr Santos & Maristela Julio Costa
2019. Bilinguismo e reconhecimento de fala no silêncio e no ruído em adultos. CoDAS 31:5 Crossref logo
Freeman, Max R., Henrike K. Blumenfeld & Viorica Marian
2017. Cross-linguistic phonotactic competition and cognitive control in bilinguals. Journal of Cognitive Psychology 29:7  pp. 783 ff. Crossref logo
GRANT, ANGELA & NANCY DENNIS
2017. Increased processing speed in young adult bilinguals: evidence from source memory judgments. Bilingualism: Language and Cognition 20:2  pp. 327 ff. Crossref logo
Grundy, John G.
2020. The effects of bilingualism on executive functions: an updated quantitative analysis. Journal of Cultural Cognitive Science 4:2  pp. 177 ff. Crossref logo
Johannessen, Janne Bondi & Michael T. Putnam
2020.  In The Cambridge Handbook of Germanic Linguistics,  pp. 783 ff. Crossref logo
Marian, Viorica, James Bartolotti, Natalia L. Daniel & Sayuri Hayakawa
2021. Spoken words activate native and non-native letter-to-sound mappings: Evidence from eye tracking. Brain and Language 223  pp. 105045 ff. Crossref logo
Wu, Struys & Lochtman
2019. Relationship Between Language Dominance and Stimulus-Stimulus or Stimulus-Response Inhibition in Uyghur-Chinese Bilinguals with an Investigation of Speed-Accuracy Trade-Offs. Behavioral Sciences 9:4  pp. 41 ff. Crossref logo
Zirnstein, Megan, Janet G van Hell & Judith F Kroll
2019. Cognitive control and language ability contribute to online reading comprehension: Implications for older adult bilinguals. International Journal of Bilingualism 23:5  pp. 971 ff. Crossref logo

This list is based on CrossRef data as of 02 november 2021. Please note that it may not be complete. Sources presented here have been supplied by the respective publishers. Any errors therein should be reported to them.

References

References

Abutalebi, J., & Green, D. W.
(2008) Control mechanisms in bilingual language production: Neural evidence from language switching studies. Language and Cognitive Processes, 23, 557–582. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Alladi, S., Bak, T. H., Duggirala, V., Surampudi, B., Shailaja, M., Shukla, A. K., Chaudhuri, J. R., & Kaul, S.
(2014) Bilingualism delays age at onset of dementia, independent of education and immigration status. Neurology, 81, 1938–1944. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Bartolotti, J., & Marian, V.
(2012) Language learning and control in monolinguals and bilinguals. Cognitive Science, 36, 1129–1147. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Bialystok, E.
(2005) Consequences of bilingualism for cognitive development. In J. F. Kroll & A. M. B. de Groot (Eds.), Handbook of Bilingualism: Psycholinguistic Approaches. Oxford, UK: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
(2007) Cognitive Effects of Bilingualism: How Linguistic Experience Leads to Cognitive Change. International Journal of Bilingual Education and Bilingualism, 10, 210–223. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
(2011) Reshaping the mind: The benefits of bilingualism. Canadian Journal of Experimental Psychology, 65, 229–235. Crossref
Bialystok, E., & Craik, F. I.M.
(2010) Cognitive and Linguistic Processing in the Bilingual Mind. Current Directions in Psychological Science, 19, 19–23. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Bialystok, E., Craik, F. I. M., Binns, M.A., Ossher, L., & Freedman, M.
(2014) Effects of bilingualism on the age of onset and progression of MCI and AD: Evidence from executive function tests. Neuropsychology, 28, 290–304. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Bialystok, E., Craik, F.I.M., & Freedman, M.
(2007) Bilingualism as a protection against the onset of symptoms of dementia.Neuropsychologia, 45, 459–464. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Bialystok, E., Craik, F. I. M., Green, D. W., & Gollan, T. H.
(2009) Bilingual minds. Psychological Science in the Public Interest, 10, 89–129. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Bialystok, E., Craik, F. I. M., Klein, R., & Viswanathan, M.
(2004) Bilingualism, aging, and cognitive control: Evidence from the Simon Task. Psychology and Aging, 19, 290–303. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Bialystok, E., Craik, F., & Luk, G.
(2008) Cognitive control and lexical access in younger and older bilinguals. Journal of Experimental Psychology. Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 34, 859–73. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Blumenfeld, H. K., & Marian, V.
(2007) Constraints on parallel activation in bilingual spoken language processing: Examining proficiency and lexical status using eye-tracking. Language and Cognitive Processes, 22, 633–660. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
(2011) Bilingualism influences inhibitory control in auditory comprehension. Cognition, 118, 245–57. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
(2013) Parallel language activation and cognitive control during spoken word recognition in bilinguals. Journal of Cognitive Psychology, 25, 547–567. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
(2014) Cognitive control in bilinguals: Advantages in stimulus-stimulus inhibition. Bilingualism: Language and Cognition, 17, 610–629. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Brink, J., & McDowd, J.
(1999) Aging and selective attention: An issue of complexity or multiple mechanisms? The Journals of Gerontology Series B: Psychological Sciences and Social Sciences, 54, P30–P33. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Craik, F. I. M., Bialystok, E., & Freedman, M.
(2010)  Delaying the onset of Alzheimer’s disease: Bilingualism as a form of cognitive reserve. Neurology, 75, 1726–1729. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Desroches, A. S., Newman, R. L., & Joanisse, M. F.
(2009) Investigating the time course of spoken word recognition: electrophysiological evidence for the influences of phonological similarity. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 21, 1893–906. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Donohue, S., Liotti, M., Perez III, R., & Woldorff, M.
(2012) Is conflict monitoring supramodal? Spatiotemporal dynamics of cognitive control processes in an auditory Stroop task. Cognitive, Affective, & Behavioral Neuroscience, 12, 1–15. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Dunn, L. M., & Dunn, L. M.
(1997) Peabody Picture Vocabulary Test (PPVT). Circle Pines, MN: American Guidance Service. Google Scholar
Dunn, L., Lugo, D., Padilla, E. & Dunn, D.
(1986) Test de Vocabulario en Imagenes Peabody. Circle Pines, Minn: American Guidance Service.Google Scholar
Foldi, N., Helm-Estabrooks, N., Redfield, J., & Nickel, D.
(2003) Perseveration in normal aging: A comparison of perseveration rates on design fluency and verbal generative tasks. Aging, Neuropsychology, and Cognition, 10, 268–280. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Garbin, G., Sanjuan, A., Forn, C., Bustamante, J. C., Rodriguez-Pujadas, A., Belloch, V., Hernandez, M., et al.
(2010) Bridging language and attention: brain basis of the impact of bilingualism on cognitive control. NeuroImage, 53, 1272–1278. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Gernsbacher, M. A.
(1993) Less skilled readers have less efficient suppression mechanisms. Psychological Science, 4, 294–298. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Gernsbacher, M., & Faust, M.
(1991) The mechanism of suppression: A component of general comprehension skill. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 17, 245–262.Google Scholar
Gold, B. T., Kim, C., Johnson, N. F., Kriscio, R. J., & Smith, C.D.
(2013) Lifelong bilingualism maintains neural efficiency for cognitive control in aging. Journal of Neuroscience, 33, 387–396. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Gollan, T. H., & Ferreira, V.S.
(2009) Should I stay or should I switch? A cost-benefit analysis of voluntary language switching in young and aging bilinguals. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 35, 640.Google Scholar
Gollan, T. H., Montoya, R. I., Cera, C., & Sandoval, T.C.
(2008) More use almost always a means a smaller frequency effect: Aging, bilingualism, and the weaker links hypothesis. Journal of Memory and Language, 58, 787–814. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Gollan, T. H., Sandoval, T., & Salmon, D.P.
(2011) Cross-language intrusion errors in aging bilinguals reveal the link between executive control and language selection. Psychological Science, 22, 1155–64. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Goral, M., Libben, G., Obler, L. K., Jarema, G., & Ohayon, K.
(2008) Lexical attrition in younger and older bilingual adults. Clinical Linguistics & Phonetics, 22, 509–522. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Gorfein, D. S., & Brown, V. R.
(2007) Saying no to inhibition: the encoding and use of words. In D. S. Gorfein & C. M. MacLeod (Eds.). The Place of Inhibition in Cognition.Washington: APA Books. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Green, D. W.
(1998) Mental control of the bilingual lexico-semantic system. Bilingualism: Language and Cognition, 1, 67–81. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Hilchey, M. D., & Klein, R.M.
(2011) Are there bilingual advantages on nonlinguistic interference tasks? Implications for the plasticity of executive control processes. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 18, 625–658. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Kane, M. J., & Engle, R.W.
(2003) Working-memory capacity and the control of attention: the contributions of goal neglect, response competition, and task set to Stroop interference. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 132, 47.Google Scholar
Kirk, N. W., Fiala, L., Scott-Brown, K. C., & Kempe, V.
(2014) No evidence for reduced Simon cost in elderly bilinguals and bidialectals. Journal of Cognitive Psychology, 26, 1–9. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Kornblum, S.
(1994) The way irrelevant dimensions are processed depends on what they overlap with: The case of Stroop-and Simon-like stimuli. Psychological Research, 56, 130–135. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Krizman, J., Skoe, E., Marian, V., & Kraus, N.
(2014) Bilingualism increases neural response consistency and attentional control: Evidence for sensory and cognitive coupling. Brain and Language, 128, 34–40. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Kroll, J. F.
(2008) Juggling two languages in one mind. Psychological Science Agenda, American Psychological Association, 22.Google Scholar
Kroll, J. F., & Bialystok, E.
(2013) Understanding the consequences of bilingualism for language processing and cognition. Journal of Cognitive Psychology, 25, 497–514. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Kroll, J. F., Bobb, S. C., Misra, M., & Guo, T.
(2008) Language selection in bilingual speech: Evidence for inhibitory processes. Acta Psychologica, 128, 416–430. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Kroll, J. F., Dussias, P. E., Bogulski, C. A., & Valdes Kroff, J. R.
(2012) Juggling two languages in one mind: What bilinguals tell us about language processing and its consequences for cognition. In B. Ross (Ed.), The Psychology of Learning and Motivation, Vol. 56 (pp. 229–262). CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Linck, J. A., Hoshino, N., & Kroll, J.F.
(2008) Cross-language lexical processes and inhibitory control. The Mental Lexicon, 3, 349.Google Scholar
Liu, X., Banich, M. T., Jacobson, B. L., & Tanabe, J.L.
(2004) Common and distinct neural substrates of attentional control in an integrated Simon and spatial Stroop task as assessed by event-related fMRI. NeuroImage, 22, 1097–1106. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Luce, P. A., & Pisoni, D.B.
(1998) Recognizing spoken words: the neighborhood activation model. Ear and Hearing, 19, 1–36. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Lustig, C., Hasher, L., & Zacks, R.T.
(2007) Inhibitory deficit theory: Recent developments in a “new view.” Inhibition in Cognition, 145–162. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Marian, V., Chabal, S., Bartolotti, J., Bradley, K., & Hernandez, A.
(2014) Differential recruitment of executive control regions during phonological competition in monolinguals and bilinguals. Brain and Language. Crossref.
Marian, V., & Spivey, M.
(2003) Competing activation in bilingual language processing: Within- and between-language competition. Bilingualism: Language and Cognition, 6, 97–115. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Martín, M.C., Macizo, P., & Bajo, T.
(2010) Time course of inhibitory processes in bilingual language processing. British Journal of Psychology, 101, 679–693. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Marslen-Wilson, W. D.
(1987) Functional parallelism in spoken word recognition. Cognition, 25, 71–102. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Mason, R., & Just, M.
(2007) Lexical ambiguity in sentence comprehension. Brain Research, 1146, 115–127. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
McClelland, J.L., & Elman, J.L.
(1986) The TRACE model of speech perception. Cognitive Psychology, 18, 1–86. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Mercier, J., Pivneva, I., & Titone, D.
(2013) Individual differences in inhibitory control relate to bilingual spoken word processing. Bilingualism: Language and Cognition, 17, 89–117. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Mishra, R., Hilchey, M., Singh, N., & Klein, R.
(2012) On the time course of exogenous cueing effects in bilinguals: Higher proficiency in a second language is associated with more rapid endogenous disengagement. The Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 65, 1502–1510. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Morales, J., Yudes, C., Gómez-Ariza, C. J., & Bajo, M.T.
(2015) Bilingualism modulates dual mechanisms of cognitive control: Evidence from ERPs. Neuropsychologia, 66, 157–169. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Paap, K. R. I., & Greenberg, Z.
(2013) There is no coherent evidence for a bilingual advantage in executive processing. Cognitive Psychology, 66, 232–258. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Prior, A., & Gollan, T.H.
(2011) Good language-switchers are good task-switchers: Evidence from Spanish-English and Mandarin-English bilinguals. Journal of the International Neuropsychological Society, 17, 682–691. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Revill, K. P., & Spieler, D. H.
(2012) The effect of lexical frequency on spoken word recognition in young and older listeners. Psychology and Aging, 27, 80–7. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Salthouse, T.A., & Meinz, E.J.
(1995) Aging, inhibition, working memory, and speed. Journal of Gerontology, 50B, 297–306. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Seidenberg, M. S., Tanenhaus, M. K., Leiman, J. M., & Bienkowski, M.
(1982) Automatic access of the meanings of ambiguous words in context: Some limitations of knowledge-based processing. Cognitive Psychology, 14, 489–537. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Singh, N., & Mishra, R.
(2014) The modulatory role of second language proficiency on performance monitoring: Evidence from a saccadic countermanding task in high and low proficient bilinguals. Frontiers in Psychology, 5, 1481.Google Scholar
Swinney, D.
1979Lexical access during sentence comprehension: (Re) consideration of context effects. Journal of Verbal Learning and Verbal Behavior, 18, 645–659 CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Weissberger, G. H., Wierenga, C. E., Bondi, M. W., & Gollan, T. H.
(2012) Partially overlapping mechanisms of language and task control in young and older bilinguals. Psychology and Aging, 27, 959.Google Scholar
West, R. & Baylis, G.
(1998) Effects of increased response dominance and contextual disintegration on the Stroop interference effect in older adults. Psychology and Aging, 13, 206–217. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Wingfield, A.
(1996) Cognitive factor in auditory performance: context, speed of processing, and constraints of memory. Journal of the American Academy of Audiology, 7, 175–182.Google Scholar