Article published In:
Aging and Bilingualism
Edited by Ellen Bialystok and Margot D. Sullivan
[Linguistic Approaches to Bilingualism 6:1/2] 2016
► pp. 86118
References
Axelrod, B.N., & Henry, R.R.
(1992) Age-related performance on the Wisconsin card sorting, similarities, and controlled oral word association tests. The Clinical Neuropsychologist, 61, 16–26. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Bialystok, E., Craik, F.I.M., & Luk, G.
(2008) Cognitive control and lexical access in younger and older bilinguals. Journal of Experimental Psychology, 341, 859–873.Google Scholar
Bialystok, E., Craik, F.I.M., Klein, R. & Viswanathan, M.
(2004) Bilingualism, aging and cognitive control: Evidence from the Simon task. Psychology and Aging, 191, 290–303. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Bolla, K. I., Gray, S., Resnick, S. M., Galante, R., & Kawas, C.
(1998) Category and letter fluency in highly educated older adults. The Clinical Neuropsychologist, 121, 330–338. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Boone, K.B.
(1999) Neuropsychological assessment of executive functions: Impact of age, education, gender, intellectual level, and vascular status on executive test scores. In B.L. Miller & J.L. Cummings (Eds.), The human frontal lobes: Functions and disorders. The science and practice of neuropsychology series (pp. 247–260). New York, NY: Guilford Press.Google Scholar
Bopp, K.L., Verhaeghen, P.
(2005) Aging and verbal memory span: A meta-analysis. Journal of Gerontology, 60B1, 223–233. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Brickman, A.M., Paul, R.H., Cohen, R.A., Williams, L.M., MacGregor, K.L., Jefferson, A.L., et al.
(2005) Category and letter verbal fluency across the adult lifespan: Relationship to EEG theta power. Archives of Clinical Neuropsychology, 201, 561–573. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Calabria, M., Branzi, F.M., Marne, P., Hernández, M., & Costa, A.
(2015) Age-related effects over bilingual language control and executive control. Bilingualism: Language and Cognition, 181, 65–78. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Calabria, M., Hernández, M., Branzi, F.M., & Costa, A.
(2011) Qualitative differences between bilingual language control and executive control: Evidence from task-switching. Frontiers in Psychology, 21, Article 399.Google Scholar
Cohen, J., MacWhinney, B., Flatt, M., & Provost, J.
(1993) PsyScope: An interactive graphical system for designing and controlling experiments in the psychology laboratory using Macintosh computers. Behavioral Research Methods, Instrumentation and Computation, 251, 257–271. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Costa, A., Hernández, M., & Sebastian-Galles, N.
(2008) Bilingualism aids conflict resolution: Evidence from the ANT task. Cognition, 1061, 59–86. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Costa, A., & Santesteban, M.
(2004) Lexical access in bilingual speech production: Evidence from language switching in highly proficient bilinguals and L2 learners. Journal of Memory and Language, 501, 491–511. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Costa, A., Hernández, M., Costa-Faidella, J., & Sebastián-Gallés, N.
(2009) On the bilingual advantage in conflict processing: Now you see it, now you don‘t. Cognition, 1131, 135–149. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Crossley, M., D'Arcy, C., & Rawson, N. S.
(1997) Letter and category fluency in community-dwelling Canadian seniors: A comparison of normal participants to those with dementia of the Alzheimer or vascular type. Journal of Clinical and Experimental Neuropsychology, 191, 52–62. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Delis, D.C., Kaplan, E., & Kramer, J.H.
(2001) Delis-Kaplan executive functioning system. San Antonio, TX: Psychology Corporation.Google Scholar
Fan, J., McCandliss, B.D., Sommer, T., Raz, A., & Posner, M.I.
(2002) Testing the efficiency and independence of attentional networks. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 141, 340–347. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Folstein, M.F., Folstein, S.E., & McHugh, P.R.
(1975) Mini-mental state: A practical method for grading the cognitive state of patients for the clinician. Journal of Psychiatric Research, 121, 189–198. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Gollan, T.H., Ferreira, V.S.
(2009) Should I stay or should I switch? A cost–benefit analysis of voluntary language switching in young and aging bilinguals. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 351, 640–665.Google Scholar
Gollan, T.H., Kleinman, D., & Wierenga, C.E.
(2014) What’s easier: Doing what you want, or being told what to do? Cued versus voluntary language and task switching. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 1431, 2167–2195. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Gollan, T.H., Montoya, R.I., Werner, G.
(2002) Semantic and letter fluency in Spanish-English bilinguals. Neuropsychology, 161, 562–576. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Gollan, T.H., Sandoval, T., & Salmon, D.P.
(2011) Cross-language intrusion errors in aging bilinguals reveal the link between executive control and language selection. Psychological Science, 221, 1155–1164. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Gollan, T.H., Schotter, E.R., Gomez, J., Murillo, M., & Rayner, K.
(2014) Multiple levels of bilingual language control: Evidence from language intrusions in reading aloud. Psychological Science, 251, 585–595. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Gollan, T.H., Weissberger, G., Runnqvist, E., Montoya, R.I., & Cera, C.M.
(2012) Self-ratings of spoken language dominance: A multi-lingual naming test (MINT) and preliminary norms for young and aging Spanish-English bilinguals. Bilingualism: Language and Cognition, 151, 594–615. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Goral, M., Campanelli, L., & Spiro III, A.
(2015) Language dominance and inhibition abilities in bilingual older adults. Bilingualism: Language and Cognition, 181, 79–89. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Green, D. W.
(1998) Mental control of the bilingual lexicosemantic system. Bilingualism: Language and Cognition, 11, 67–81. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Hasher, L. & Zacks, R.T.
(1988) Working memory, comprehension, and aging: A review and a new view. The Psychology of Learning and Motivation, 221, 193–225. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Henry, J. D., Crawford, J. R., & Phillips, L. H.
(2004) Verbal fluency performance in dementia of the Alzheimer’s type: A meta-analysis. Neuropsychologia, 421, 1212–1222. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Hernandez, A. E. & Kohnert, K. J.
(1999) Aging and language switching in bilinguals. Aging, Neuropsychology, and Cognition, 61, 69–83. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Hernández, M., Martin, C.D., Barceló, F., & Costa, A.
(2013) Where is the bilingual advantage in task-switching? Journal of Memory and Language, 691, 257–276. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Kleinman, D. & Gollan, T.H.
submitted). Speaking two languages for the price of one: Bypassing language control mechanisms via accessibility-driven switches.
Kozora, E., Cullum, C.M.
(1995) Generative naming in normal aging: Total output and qualitative changes using phonemic and semantic constraints. The Clinical Neuropsychologist, 91, 313–320. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Ljungberg, J.K., Hansson, P., Andrés, P., Josefsson, M., & Nilsson, L.-G.
(2013) A longitudinal study of memory advantages in bilinguals. PLoS ONE, 81, e73029. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Luo, L., Luk, G., & Bialystok, E.
(2010) Effect of language proficiency and executive control on verbal fluency performance in bilinguals. Cognition, 1141, 29–41. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Martin, A., Wiggs, C.L., Lalonde, F., & Mack, C
(1994) Word retrieval to letter and semantic cues:A double dissociations in normal subjects using interference tasks. Neuropsychologia, 321, 1487–1492. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Mattis, S.
(1988) Dementia rating scale: Professional manual. Odessa, FL: Psychological Assessment Resources.Google Scholar
Meuter, R. F., & Allport, A.
(1999) Bilingual language switching in naming: Asymmetrical costs of language selection. Journal of Memory and Language, 401, 25–40. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Myerson, J., Hale, S., Wagstaff, D., Poon, L.W., & Smith, G.A.
(1990) The information-loss model: A mathematical theory of age-related cognitive slowing. Psychological Review, 971, 475–487. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Paap, K.R., & Greenberg, Z.I.
(2013) There is no coherent evidence for a bilingual advantage in executive processing. Cognitive Psychology, 661, 232–258. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Portocarrero, J.S., Burright, R.G., & Donovick. P.J.
(2007) Vocabulary and verbal fluency of bilingual and monolingual college students. Archives of Clinical Neuropsychology, 221, 415–422. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Poulisse, N.
(1999) Slips of the tongue: Speech errors in first and second language production. Amsterdam, The Netherlands: John Benjamins. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Prior, A. & Gollan, T.H.
(2011) Good language-switchers are good task-switchers: Evidence from Spanish–English and Mandarin–English bilinguals. Journal of the International Neuropsychological Society, 171, 682–691. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Prior, A., & Gollan, T.H.
(2013) The elusive link between language control and executive control: A case of limited transfer, The Journal of Cognitive Psychology, 251, 622–645. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Rascovsky, K., Salmon, D. P., Hansen, L. A., Thal, L. J., & Galasko, D.
(2007) Disparate letter and semantic category fluency deficits in autopsy-confirmed frontotemporal dementia and Alzheimer's disease. Neuropsychology, 211, 20–30. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Reitan, RM.
(1958) Validity of the Trail Making Test to organic brain damage. Perceptual Motor Skills, 81, 271–276. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Rosselli, M., Ardila, A., Araujo, K., Weekes, V.A., Caracciolo, V., Padilla, M., et al.
(2000) Verbal fluency and repetition skills in healthy older Spanish–English bilinguals. Applied Neuropsychology, 71, 1–24. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Salthouse, T.A.
(1985) A theory of cognitive aging. Amsterdam: North Holland.Google Scholar
Sandoval, T.
(2010) The role of control in bilingual verbal fluency: Evidence from aging and Alzheimer’s Disease. Unpublished doctoral dissertation.Google Scholar
Sandoval, T.C., Gollan, T.H., Ferreira, V.S., & Salmon, D.P.
(2010) What causes the bilingual disadvantage in verbal fluency: The dual-task analogy. Bilingualism: Language and Cognition, 131, 231–252. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Simon, J.R., & Ruddell, A.P.
(1967) Auditory S-R compatibility: The effect of an irrelevant cue on information processing. Journal of Applied Psychology, 511, 300–304. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Strauss, E., Sherman, E.M.S., & Spreen, O.
(2006) A compendium of neuropsychological tests: Administration, norms, and commentary (Third Ed.). New York: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Tao, L, Taft, M. & Gollan, T.H.
(2015) The bilingual switching advantage: Sometimes related to bilingual proficiency, sometimes not. The Journal of the International Neuropsychological Society, 211, 531–544. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Tombaugh, T. N., Kozak, J., & Rees, L.
(1999) Normative data stratified by age and education for two measures of verbal fluency: FAS and animal naming. Archives of Clinical Neuropsychology, 141, 167–177.Google Scholar
Tomer, R., Levin, B. E.
(1993) Differential effects of aging on two verbal fluency tasks. Perceptual and Motor Skills, 761, 465–466. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Van Assche, E., Duyck, W., & Gollan, T.H.
(2013) Whole-language and item-specific control in bilingual language production. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 391, 1781–1792.Google Scholar
Van der Lubbe, R. & Verleger, R.
(2002) Aging and the Simon task. Psychophysiology, 391, 100–110. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Van Hell, J. G., & Dijkstra, T.
(2002) Foreign language knowledge can influence native language performance in exclusively native contexts. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 91, 780–789. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Verhaeghen, P.
(2011) Aging and executive control: Reports of a demise greatly exaggerated. Psychological Science, 201, 174–180.Google Scholar
Verreyt, N., Woumans, E., Vandelanotte, D., Szmalec, A., & Duyck, W.
(2016) The influence of language switching experience on the bilingual executive control advantage. Bilingualism: Language and Cognition, 19, 181-190. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Wasylyshyn, C., Verhaeghen, P., & Sliwinski, M.
(2011) Aging and task switching. A meta-analysis. Psychology and Aging, 261, 15–20. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Wechsler, D.
(1997) Wechsler adult intelligence scale (Third Ed.). San Antonio, TX: The Psychological Corporation.Google Scholar
Weissberger, G. H., Wierenga, C. E., Bondi, M. W., & Gollan, T. H.
(2012) Partially overlapping mechanisms of language and task control in young and older bilinguals. Psychology and Aging, 271, 959–974. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Zacks, R.T., & Hasher, L.
(1994) Directed ignoring: Inhibitory regulation of working memory. In D. Dagenbach & T.H. Carr (Eds.), Inhibitory Processes in Attention, Memory, and Language (pp. 241–264). San Diego, CA: Academic Press.Google Scholar
Cited by

Cited by 14 other publications

Buğa, Duygu
2018. Central Language Hypothesis. In Psycholinguistics and Cognition in Language Processing [Advances in Linguistics and Communication Studies, ],  pp. 1 ff. DOI logo
D'Alessandro, Roberta, David Natvig & Michael T. Putnam
2021. Addressing Challenges in Formal Research on Moribund Heritage Languages: A Path Forward. Frontiers in Psychology 12 DOI logo
de Bruin, Angela, Heidi Kressel & Daisy Hemmings
2023. A comparison of language control while switching within versus between languages in younger and older adults. Scientific Reports 13:1 DOI logo
DECLERCK, MATHIEU, KRISTIN LEMHÖFER & JONATHAN GRAINGER
2017. Bilingual language interference initiates error detection: Evidence from language intrusions. Bilingualism: Language and Cognition 20:5  pp. 1010 ff. DOI logo
Gollan, Tamar H., Alena Stasenko, Chuchu Li & David P. Salmon
2017. Bilingual language intrusions and other speech errors in Alzheimer’s disease. Brain and Cognition 118  pp. 27 ff. DOI logo
Hejazi, Zahra, Jungna Kim, Teresa Signorelli Pisano, Yasmine Ouchikh, Aviva Lerman & Loraine K. Obler
2019. Brain‐based Challenges of Second Language Learning in Older Adulthood. In The Handbook of the Neuroscience of Multilingualism,  pp. 408 ff. DOI logo
Johannessen, Janne Bondi & Michael T. Putnam
2020. Heritage Germanic Languages in North America. In The Cambridge Handbook of Germanic Linguistics,  pp. 783 ff. DOI logo
Liu, Hong
2023. Attention: The Cognitive Effects of Learning to Read in Arabic by Chinese Learners at an Old Age. SAGE Open 13:3 DOI logo
Mishra, Ramesh Kumar, Manasa Padmanabhuni, Pratik Bhandari, Shiji Viswambharan & Seema Gorur Prasad
2019. Language proficiency does not modulate executive control in older bilinguals. Aging, Neuropsychology, and Cognition 26:6  pp. 920 ff. DOI logo
Phani Krishna, P., S. Arulmozi, Shiva Ram Male & Ramesh Kumar Mishra
2023. Are Older Bilinguals’ Better in Metaphor Generation?. Journal of Psycholinguistic Research 52:4  pp. 1183 ff. DOI logo
Putnam, Michael T., Tanja Kupisch & Diego Pascual y Cabo
2018. Chapter 12. Different situations, similar outcomes. In Bilingual Cognition and Language [Studies in Bilingualism, 54],  pp. 251 ff. DOI logo
Shishkin, Elena & Peter Ecke
2018. Language Dominance, Verbal Fluency, and Language Control in two Groups of Russian–English Bilinguals. Languages 3:3  pp. 27 ff. DOI logo
STRUYS, ESLI, EVY WOUMANS, SOUDABEH NOUR, OLGA KEPINSKA & MAURITS VAN DEN NOORT
2019. A domain-general monitoring account of language switching in recognition tasks: Evidence for adaptive control. Bilingualism: Language and Cognition 22:3  pp. 606 ff. DOI logo

This list is based on CrossRef data as of 3 april 2024. Please note that it may not be complete. Sources presented here have been supplied by the respective publishers. Any errors therein should be reported to them.