To be specified published in:
Aging and Bilingualism
Edited by Ellen Bialystok and Margot D. Sullivan
[Linguistic Approaches to Bilingualism 6:1/2] 2016
► pp. 147170
Cited by

Cited by 2 other publications

Barker, Ryan M. & Ellen Bialystok
2019. Processing differences between monolingual and bilingual young adults on an emotion n-back task. Brain and Cognition 134  pp. 29 ff. Crossref logo
Ma, Xie, Xiao Ma, Peng Li & Yan Liu
2020. Differences in Working Memory With Emotional Distraction Between Proficient and Non-proficient Bilinguals. Frontiers in Psychology 11 Crossref logo

This list is based on CrossRef data as of 25 may 2021. Please note that it may not be complete. Sources presented here have been supplied by the respective publishers. Any errors therein should be reported to them.

References

References

Adesope, O.O., Lavin, T., Thompson, T., & Ungerleider, C.
(2010) A systematic review and meta-analysis of the cognitive correlates of bilingualism. Review of Educational Research, 80 (2), 207–245. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Bialystok, E.
(2015) Bilingualism and the development of executive function: The role of attention. Child Development Perspectives, 9 (2), 117–121. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Bialystok, E., Craik, F.I.M., Green, D.W., & Gollan, T.H.
(2009) Bilingual minds. Psychological Science in the Public Interest, 10, 89–129. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Bialystok, E., Craik, F., & Luk, G.
(2008) Cognitive control and lexical access in younger and older bilinguals. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 34 (4), 859–873. Crossref
Bialystok, E., Craik, F. I. M., Klein, R., & Viswanathan, M.
(2004) Bilingualism, aging, and cognitive control: Evidence from the Simon task. Psychology and Aging, 19(2), 290–303. Crossref
Bialystok, E., & Feng, X.
(2009) Language proficiency and executive control in proactive interference: Evidence from monolingual and bilingual children and adults. Brain & Language, 109, 93–100. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Bialystok, E., & Luk, G.
(2012) Receptive vocabulary differences in monolingual and bilingual adults. Bilingualism: Language and Cognition, 15, 397–401. Crossref
Bialystok, E., Luk, G., Peets, K.F., & Yang, S.
(2010) Receptive vocabulary differences in monolingual and bilingual children. Bilingualism: Language and Cognition, 13 (4), 525–531. Crossref
Bialystok, E., Poarch, G. J., Luo, L., & Craik, F. I. M.
(2014) Effects of bilingualism and aging on executive function and working memory. Psychology and Aging, 29 (3), 696–705. Crossref.Google Scholar
Das-Smaal, E., de Jong, P. F., & Koopmans, J. R.
(1993) Working memory, attentional regulation and the star counting test. Personality and Individual Differences, 14(6), 815–824. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Eriksson, J., Vogel, E.K., Lansner, A., Bergström, F., & Nyberg, L.
(2015) Neurocognitive architecture of working memory. Neuron, 88 (1), 33–46. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Fernandes, M.A., Craik, F.I.M., Bialystok, E., & Kreuger, S.
(2007) Effects of bilingualism, aging, and semantic relatedness on memory under divided attention. Canadian Journal of Experimental Psychology, 6 (2), 128–141. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Gollan, T.H., Montoya, R.I., Cera, C.M., & & Sandoval, T.C.
(2008) More use almost always means smaller a frequency effect: Aging, bilingualism, and the weaker links hypothesis. Journal of Memory and Language, 58, 787–814. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Gollan, T.H., & Silverberg, N.B.
(2001) Tip-of-the-tongue states in Hebrew-English bilinguals. Bilingualism: Language and Cognition, 4, 63–83. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Grundy, J.G., & Timmer, K.
submitted). Bilingualism and working memory capacity: A comprehensive meta-analysis.
Hasher, L., Zacks, R. T., & May, C. P.
(1999) Inhibitory control, circadian arousal, and age. In D. Gopher and A. Koriat (Eds.), Attention and performance XVII: Cognitive regulation of performance: Interaction of theory and application. Attention and performance (pp. 653–675). Cambridge, MA: The MIT Press.Google Scholar
Ivanova, I., & Costa, A.
(2008) Does bilingualism hamper lexical access in speech production? Acta Psychologica, 127, 277–288. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Jacoby. L.L.
(1991) A process dissociation framework: Separating automatic from intentional uses of memory. Journal of Memory and Language, 30 (5), 513–541. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Jonides, J., Marshuetz, C., Smith, E.E., Reuter-Lorenz, P.A., Koeppe, R.A.
(2000) Age differences in behavior and PET activation reveal differences in interference resolution in verbal working memory. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 12 (1), 188–196. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Jonides, J., & Nee, D. E.
(2006) Brain mechanisms of proactive interference in working memory. Neuroscience, 139, 181–193. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Kroll, J. F., Dussias, P. E., Bogulski, C. A., & Valdes-Kroff, J.
(2012) Juggling two languages in one mind: What bilinguals tell us about language processing and its consequences for cognition. In B. Ross (Ed.), The Psychology of Learning and Motivation (Vol. 56, pp. 229–262). San Diego: Academic Press.Google Scholar
Linck, J.A., Osthus, P., Koeth, J.T., & Bunting, M.F.
(2014) Working memory and second language comprehension: A meta-analysis. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 21, 861–883. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Lu, C.H, & Proctor, R.W.
(1995) The influence of irrelevant location information on performance: A review of the Simon and spatial Stroop effects. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 2, 174–207. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Luk, G. & Bialystok, E.
(2013) Bilingualism is not a categorical variable: Interaction between language proficiency and usage. Journal of Cognitive Psychology, 25, 605–621. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Luo, L., Craik, F. I. M., Moreno, S., & Bialystok, E.
(2013) Bilingualism interacts with domain in a working memory task: Evidence from aging. Psychology and Aging, 28(1), 28–34. Crossref
May, C., Hasher, L., & Kane, M.J.
(1999) The role of interference in memory span. Memory & Cognition, 27, 759–767. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Oberauer, K., Süss, H.M., Schulze, R., Wilhelm, O., & Wittman, W.W.
(2000) Working memory capacity- facets of a cognitive ability construct. Personality and Individual Differences, 29, 1017–1045. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Shipley, W. C., Gruber, C. P., Martin, T. A., & Klein, A. M.
(2009) Shipley-2 Manual. Los Angeles, CA: Western Psychological Services.Google Scholar
van Merrienboer, J., & Sweller, J.
(2005) Cognitive load theory and complex learning: Recent developments and future directions. Educational Psychology Review, 17, 147–177. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Wodniecka, Z., Craik, F.I.M., Luo, L., & Bialystok, E.
(2010) Does bilingualism help memory? Competing effects of verbal ability and executive control. International Journal of Bilingual Education and Bilingualism, 13 (5), 575–595. CrossrefGoogle Scholar