Studies across the lifespan have revealed modifications in executive control (EC) from bilingualism, but studies of working memory (WM), a key aspect of EC, have produced varied results. Healthy older (M = 71.0 years) and younger participants (M = 21.1 years) who were monolingual or bilingual, performed working memory tasks that varied in their demands for EC. Tasks included a star counting task, a flanker task, and a nonverbal recent probe memory task. Bilinguals performed similarly to monolinguals on the star counting task after controlling for differences in vocabulary. Monolinguals were faster than bilinguals on the flanker task with only age group differences significant for the WM manipulation. Bilinguals were faster than monolinguals on the nonverbal recent probe memory task, particularly for the condition that included proactive interference. The interpretation is that better bilingual performance in nonverbal working memory tasks is linked to the need for executive control.
Adesope, O.O., Lavin, T., Thompson, T., & Ungerleider, C. (2010). A systematic review and meta-analysis of the cognitive correlates of bilingualism. Review of Educational Research, 80 (2), 207–245.
Bialystok, E. (2015). Bilingualism and the development of executive function: The role of attention. Child Development Perspectives, 9 (2), 117–121.
Bialystok, E., Craik, F.I.M., Green, D.W., & Gollan, T.H. (2009). Bilingual minds. Psychological Science in the Public Interest, 101, 89–129.
Bialystok, E., Craik, F., & Luk, G. (2008). Cognitive control and lexical access in younger and older bilinguals. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 34 (4), 859–873.
Bialystok, E., Craik, F. I. M., Klein, R., & Viswanathan, M. (2004). Bilingualism, aging, and cognitive control: Evidence from the Simon task. Psychology and Aging, 19(2), 290–303.
Bialystok, E., & Feng, X. (2009). Language proficiency and executive control in proactive interference: Evidence from monolingual and bilingual children and adults. Brain & Language, 1091, 93–100.
Bialystok, E., & Luk, G. (2012). Receptive vocabulary differences in monolingual and bilingual adults. Bilingualism: Language and Cognition, 151, 397–401.
Bialystok, E., Luk, G., Peets, K.F., & Yang, S. (2010). Receptive vocabulary differences in monolingual and bilingual children. Bilingualism: Language and Cognition, 13 (4), 525–531.
Bialystok, E., Poarch, G. J., Luo, L., & Craik, F. I. M. (2014). Effects of bilingualism and aging on executive function and working memory. Psychology and Aging, 29 (3), 696–705. .
Das-Smaal, E., de Jong, P. F., & Koopmans, J. R. (1993). Working memory, attentional regulation and the star counting test. Personality and Individual Differences, 14(6), 815–824.
Eriksson, J., Vogel, E.K., Lansner, A., Bergström, F., & Nyberg, L. (2015). Neurocognitive architecture of working memory. Neuron, 88 (1), 33–46.
Fernandes, M.A., Craik, F.I.M., Bialystok, E., & Kreuger, S. (2007). Effects of bilingualism, aging, and semantic relatedness on memory under divided attention. Canadian Journal of Experimental Psychology, 6 (2), 128–141.
Gollan, T.H., Montoya, R.I., Cera, C.M., & & Sandoval, T.C., (2008). More use almost always means smaller a frequency effect: Aging, bilingualism, and the weaker links hypothesis. Journal of Memory and Language, 581, 787–814.
Gollan, T.H., & Silverberg, N.B. (2001). Tip-of-the-tongue states in Hebrew-English bilinguals. Bilingualism: Language and Cognition, 41, 63–83.
Grundy, J.G., & Timmer, K. (submitted). Bilingualism and working memory capacity: A comprehensive meta-analysis.
Hasher, L., Zacks, R. T., & May, C. P. (1999). Inhibitory control, circadian arousal, and age. In D. Gopher and A. Koriat (Eds.), Attention and performance XVII: Cognitive regulation of performance: Interaction of theory and application. Attention and performance (pp. 653–675). Cambridge, MA: The MIT Press.
Ivanova, I., & Costa, A. (2008). Does bilingualism hamper lexical access in speech production?Acta Psychologica, 1271, 277–288.
Jacoby. L.L. (1991). A process dissociation framework: Separating automatic from intentional uses of memory. Journal of Memory and Language, 30 (5), 513–541.
Jonides, J., Marshuetz, C., Smith, E.E., Reuter-Lorenz, P.A., Koeppe, R.A. (2000). Age differences in behavior and PET activation reveal differences in interference resolution in verbal working memory. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 12 (1), 188–196.
Jonides, J., & Nee, D. E. (2006). Brain mechanisms of proactive interference in working memory. Neuroscience, 1391, 181–193.
Kroll, J. F., Dussias, P. E., Bogulski, C. A., & Valdes-Kroff, J. (2012). Juggling two languages in one mind: What bilinguals tell us about language processing and its consequences for cognition. In B. Ross (Ed.), The Psychology of Learning and Motivation (Vol. 561, pp. 229–262). San Diego: Academic Press.
Linck, J.A., Osthus, P., Koeth, J.T., & Bunting, M.F. (2014). Working memory and second language comprehension: A meta-analysis. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 211, 861–883.
Lu, C.H, & Proctor, R.W. (1995). The influence of irrelevant location information on performance: A review of the Simon and spatial Stroop effects. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 21, 174–207.
Luk, G. & Bialystok, E. (2013). Bilingualism is not a categorical variable: Interaction between language proficiency and usage. Journal of Cognitive Psychology, 251, 605–621.
Luo, L., Craik, F. I. M., Moreno, S., & Bialystok, E. (2013). Bilingualism interacts with domain in a working memory task: Evidence from aging. Psychology and Aging, 28(1), 28–34.
May, C., Hasher, L., & Kane, M.J. (1999). The role of interference in memory span. Memory & Cognition, 271, 759–767.
Oberauer, K., Süss, H.M., Schulze, R., Wilhelm, O., & Wittman, W.W. (2000). Working memory capacity- facets of a cognitive ability construct. Personality and Individual Differences, 291, 1017–1045.
Shipley, W. C., Gruber, C. P., Martin, T. A., & Klein, A. M., (2009). Shipley-2 Manual. Los Angeles, CA: Western Psychological Services.
van Merrienboer, J., & Sweller, J. (2005). Cognitive load theory and complex learning: Recent developments and future directions. Educational Psychology Review, 171, 147–177.
Wodniecka, Z., Craik, F.I.M., Luo, L., & Bialystok, E. (2010). Does bilingualism help memory? Competing effects of verbal ability and executive control. International Journal of Bilingual Education and Bilingualism, 13 (5), 575–595.
Cited by (9)
Cited by nine other publications
Vassiliu, Chrysoula, Victoria Leong & Henriette Hendriks
2024. The Influence of Multilingual Experience on Executive Function and Structure Learning: Effects in Young Adults in the UK and Singapore. Languages 9:4 ► pp. 136 ff.
Liu, Hong
2023. Attention: The Cognitive Effects of Learning to Read in Arabic by Chinese Learners at an Old Age. Sage Open 13:3
2022. The role of bilingualism in executive functions in healthy older adults: A systematic review. International Journal of Bilingualism 26:4 ► pp. 426 ff.
Voits, Toms, Holly Robson, Jason Rothman & Christos Pliatsikas
2022. The effects of bilingualism on hippocampal volume in ageing bilinguals. Brain Structure and Function 227:3 ► pp. 979 ff.
李, 婷
2022. The Effects of Second Language Acquisition on Creative Thinking and Its Neural Mechanisms. Advances in Psychology 12:07 ► pp. 2385 ff.
Calvo, Noelia & Ellen Bialystok
2021. Electrophysiological signatures of attentional control in bilingual processing: Evidence from proactive interference. Brain and Language 222 ► pp. 105027 ff.
Ma, Xie, Xiao Ma, Peng Li & Yan Liu
2020. Differences in Working Memory With Emotional Distraction Between Proficient and Non-proficient Bilinguals. Frontiers in Psychology 11
Barker, Ryan M. & Ellen Bialystok
2019. Processing differences between monolingual and bilingual young adults on an emotion n-back task. Brain and Cognition 134 ► pp. 29 ff.
This list is based on CrossRef data as of 21 october 2024. Please note that it may not be complete. Sources presented here have been supplied by the respective publishers.
Any errors therein should be reported to them.