Article published In:
Linguistic Approaches to Bilingualism
Vol. 8:2 (2018) ► pp.193216
References
Bardel, C., & Falk, Y.
(2007) The role of the second language in third language acquisition: The case of Germanic syntax. Second Language Research, 231, 459–484. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Bialystok, E.
(2005) Consequences of bilingualism for cognitive development. In J.F. Kroll & A. M. B. de Groot (Eds), Handbook of bilingualism (pp. 417–432). Oxford: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Cabrelli Amaro, J., Amaro J. F., & Rothman, J.
(2015) The relationship between L3 transfer and structural similarity across development: Raising across an experiencer in Brazilian Portuguese. In H. Peukert (Ed), Transfer effects in multilingual language development (pp. 21–52). Amsterdam/Philadelphia: John Benjamins Publishing Company.Google Scholar
Cinque, G.
(1995) Italian syntax and universal grammar. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Dabir-Moghaddam, M.
(2006) Internal and external forces in typology: Evidence from Iranian languages. Journal of Universal Language, 71, 29–47. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Dopke, S.
(1992) One parent one language: An interactional approach. Amsterdam: John Benjamins. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Falk, Y., & Bardel, C.
(2011) Object pronouns in German L3 syntax: Evidence for the L2 status factor. Second Language Research, 271, 59–82. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Fallah, N., Jabbari, A. A, & Fazilatfar, A. M.
(2016) Source(s) of syntactic CLI: The case of L3 acquisition of English possessives by Mazanderani-Persian bilinguals. Second Language Research, 321, 225–245. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Flynn, S., Foley, C., & Vinnitskaya, I.
(2004) The Cumulative-Enhancement Model for language acquisition: Comparing adults’ and children’s patterns of development in first, second and third language acquisition of relative clauses. The International Journal of Multilingualism, 11, 3–16. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Ghomeshi, J.
(1997) Non-projecting nouns and the Ezafe construction in Persian. Natural Language and Linguistic Theory, 151, 729–788. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Giancaspro. D., Halloran, B., & Iverson, M.
(2015) Examining L3 transfer: The acquisition of differential object marking in L3 Brazilian Portuguese. Bilingualism: Language and Cognition, 181, 191–207. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Green, D. W.
(1998) Mental control of the bilingual lexico-semantic system. Bilingualism: Language and Cognition, 11, 67–81. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Grosjean, F.
(1982) Life with two languages: An introduction to bilingualism. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.Google Scholar
Hawkins, R., & Chan, Y-HC.
(1997) The partial availability of Universal Grammar in second language acquisition: The ‘failed functional features hypothesis’. Second Language Research, 131, 187–226. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Hermas, A.
(2010) Language acquisition as computational resetting: Verb movement in L3 initial state. International Journal of Multilingualism, 71, 343–362. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
(2014a) Multilingual transfer: L1 morphosyntax in L3 English. International Journal of Language Studies, 81, 1–24.Google Scholar
(2014b) Restrictive relatives in L3 English: L1 transfer and ultimate attainment convergence. Australian Journal of Linguistics, 341, 361–387. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Iverson, M.
(2009) N-drop at the initial state of L3 Portuguese: Comparing simultaneous and additive bilinguals of English/Spanish. In A. Pires & J. Rothman (Eds.), Minimalist inquiries into child and adult language acquisition: Case studies across Portuguese (pp. 221–244). Berlin: Mouton de Gruyter.Google Scholar
Kayne, R.
(1994) The antisymmetry of syntax. Cambridge, Mass: MIT Press.Google Scholar
Kellerman, E.
(1983) Now you see it, now you don’t. In S. Gass & L. Selinker (Eds.), Language transfer in language learning (pp. 112–134). Rowley, MA: Newbury House.Google Scholar
Klausen, T., Subritzky, M., & Hayashi, M.
(1993) Initial production of inflections in bilingual children. In D. J. Messer, & G. J. Turner (Eds.), Critical influences on child language acquisition and development (pp. 65–92). New York: St Martin’s Press. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Larson, R. K.
(2009) Chinese as a reverse ezafe language. Yuyaxue Luncong (Journal of Linguistics), 391, 30–85.Google Scholar
Lozano, C.
(2003) Focus pronouns and word order in the acquisition of L2 and L3 Spanish. Ph.D. dissertation, University of Essex, England.Google Scholar
Moinzadeh, A.
(2006) The Ezafe phrase in Persian: How complements are added to N°s and A°s? Journal of Social Sciences & Humanities of Shiraz University, 231, 45–57.Google Scholar
Na Ranong, S., & Leung, Y- K.
(2009) Null objects in L1 Thai-L2 English-L3 Chinese: An empirical take on a theoretical problem. In Y-K. Leung (Ed.), Third language acquisition and universal grammar (pp. 162–191). Clevedon, UK: Multilingual Matters. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Paradis, M.
(2004) A neurolinguistic theory of bilingualism. Amsterdam: John Benjamins. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
(2007) L1 attrition features predicted by a neurolinguistic theory of bilingualism. In B. Köpke, M. S. Schmid, M. Keijzer, & S. Dostert (Eds.), Language attrition: Theoretical perspectives (pp. 121–134). Amsterdam: John Benjamins. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Radford, A.
(1997) Syntactic theory and the structure of English. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
(2004) Minimalist syntax: Exploring the structure of English. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Rothman, J.
(2010) On the typological economy of syntactic transfer: Word order and relative clause high/low attachment preference in L3 Brazilian Portuguese. International Review of Applied Linguistics in Teaching (IRAL), 481, 245–273.Google Scholar
(2011) L3 syntactic transfer selectivity and typological determinacy: The Typological Primacy Model. Second Language Research, 271, 107–127. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
(2013) Cognitive economy, non-redundancy and typological primacy in L3 acquisition: Evidence from initial stages of L3 Romance. In S. Baauw, F. Dirjkoningen, & M. Pinto (Eds.), Romance languages and linguistic theory (pp. 217–247). Amsterdam: Benjamins.Google Scholar
(2015) Linguistic and cognitive motivations for the Typological Primacy Model (TPM) of third language (L3) transfer: Timing of acquisition and proficiency considered. Bilingualism: Language and Cognition, 181, 1–12. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Rothman, J., & Cabrelli Amaro, J.
(2010) What variables condition syntactic transfer? A look at the L3 initial state. Second Language Research, 261, 189–218. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Rothman, J., & Halloran, B.
(2013) Formal linguistic approaches to L3/Ln acquisition: A focus on morphosyntactic transfer in adult multilingualism. Annual Review of Applied Linguistics, 331, 51–67. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Rothman, J., & Iverson, M.
(2013) Strong islands and null objects in L2 Spanish of Brazilian Portuguese natives: Do you know the learners who drop___? Studies in Second Language Acquisition, 35(4), 589–618. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Samiian, V.
(1983) Structure of phrasal categories in Persian: An X-Bar analysis. (Unpublished doctoral dissertation). UCLA, USA.Google Scholar
Schlyter, S.
(1993) The weaker language in bilingual Swedish-French children. In K. Hyltenstam & A. Viberg (Eds.), Progression and regression in language (pp. 289–308). Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Treffers-Daller, J.
(2011) Operationalizing and measuring language dominance. International Journal of Bilingualism, 15(2), 147–163. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Cited by

Cited by 20 other publications

Agustín-Llach, María-del-Pilar
2024. Some good news for the school: the levelling effect of the EFL class in FL vocabulary production and FL categorization of autochthonous learners and heritage speaker learners. European Journal of Applied Linguistics 0:0 DOI logo
Angelovska, Tanja, Dietmar Roehm & Sabrina Weinmüller
2023. Uncovering transfer effects of dominance and proficiency in L3 English acquisition using the visual moving window paradigm and grammaticality judgments. Applied Linguistics Review 14:1  pp. 115 ff. DOI logo
Cabrelli, Jennifer & Carrie Pichan
2021. Initial phonological transfer in L3 Brazilian Portuguese and Italian. Linguistic Approaches to Bilingualism 11:2  pp. 131 ff. DOI logo
González Alonso, Jorge & Eloi Puig-Mayenco
2021. You know more than you say. Linguistic Approaches to Bilingualism 11:1  pp. 54 ff. DOI logo
González Alonso, Jorge, Eloi Puig-Mayenco, Antonio Fábregas, Adel Chaouch-Orozco, Jason Rothman & Niels O. Schiller
2021. On the status of transfer in adult third language acquisition of early bilinguals. PLOS ONE 16:3  pp. e0247976 ff. DOI logo
Jamali, Maryam, Ali Akbar Jabbari & Mohammad Hasan Razmi
2023. Acquisition of attributive adjectives and noun adjuncts by L3 learners of French and German: further evidence for the typological primacy model (TPM). International Review of Applied Linguistics in Language Teaching 61:3  pp. 851 ff. DOI logo
Jo, Kyuhee, Kitaek Kim & Hyunwoo Kim
2021. Children's interpretation of negation and quantifier scope in L3 English. Bilingualism: Language and Cognition 24:3  pp. 427 ff. DOI logo
Kolb, Nadine, Natalia Mitrofanova & Marit Westergaard
2023. Structural similarity in third language acquisition. Linguistic Approaches to Bilingualism 13:5  pp. 607 ff. DOI logo
Kong, Stano
2023. The acquisition of L3 French present simple and present progressive by adult L1 Chinese speakers of L2 English. International Review of Applied Linguistics in Language Teaching 61:2  pp. 655 ff. DOI logo
Mitrofanova, Natalia, Evelina Leivada & Marit Westergaard
2023. Crosslinguistic influence in L3 acquisition. Linguistic Approaches to Bilingualism 13:5  pp. 717 ff. DOI logo
Perpiñán, Sílvia & Silvina Montrul
2023. Does your regional variety help you acquire an additional language?. Linguistic Approaches to Bilingualism 13:5  pp. 663 ff. DOI logo
Puig-Mayenco, Eloi, Jorge González Alonso & Jason Rothman
2020. A systematic review of transfer studies in third language acquisition. Second Language Research 36:1  pp. 31 ff. DOI logo
Puig-Mayenco, Eloi, David Miller & Jason Rothman
2018. Chapter 9. Language dominance and transfer selection in L3 acquisition. In Meaning and Structure in Second Language Acquisition [Studies in Bilingualism, 55],  pp. 229 ff. DOI logo
Puig-Mayenco, Eloi, Jason Rothman & Susagna Tubau
2022. Language dominance in the previously acquired languages modulates rate of third language (L3) development over time: a longitudinal study. International Journal of Bilingual Education and Bilingualism 25:5  pp. 1641 ff. DOI logo
Ramos Feijoo, Jon & María del Pilar García Mayo
2023. The acquisition of relative clauses by Spanish-Basque learners of L3 English: does dominance play a role?. International Review of Applied Linguistics in Language Teaching 61:3  pp. 1051 ff. DOI logo
Rothman, Jason, Jorge González Alonso & Eloi Puig-Mayenco
2019. Third Language Acquisition and Linguistic Transfer, DOI logo
Savelieva, Tatiana & Yulia Rodina
Schwartz, Bonnie D. & Rex A. Sprouse
2021. The Full Transfer/Full Access model and L3 cognitive states. Linguistic Approaches to Bilingualism 11:1  pp. 1 ff. DOI logo
Syed, Nasir Abbas & Shah Bibi
2024. VOT for plosives in the indigenous languages of Balochistan. Journal of Second Language Studies 7:1  pp. 157 ff. DOI logo
Vargas, Janina Camille & María del Pilar García Mayo
2022. Lexical Crosslinguistic Influence in L3 Spanish by Tagalog–English Bilinguals. Languages 7:3  pp. 213 ff. DOI logo

This list is based on CrossRef data as of 3 april 2024. Please note that it may not be complete. Sources presented here have been supplied by the respective publishers. Any errors therein should be reported to them.