Introduction published in:
Aging and Bilingualism
Edited by Ellen Bialystok and Margot D. Sullivan
[Linguistic Approaches to Bilingualism 6:1/2] 2016
► pp. 18
Cited by

Cited by 8 other publications

Alladi, Suvarna, Thomas H. Bak, Mekala Shailaja, Divyaraj Gollahalli, Amulya Rajan, Bapiraju Surampudi, Michael Hornberger, Vasanta Duggirala, Jaydip Ray Chaudhuri & Subhash Kaul
2017. Bilingualism delays the onset of behavioral but not aphasic forms of frontotemporal dementia. Neuropsychologia 99  pp. 207 ff. Crossref logo
Anderson, John A. E., Lorinda Mak, Aram Keyvani Chahi & Ellen Bialystok
2018. The language and social background questionnaire: Assessing degree of bilingualism in a diverse population. Behavior Research Methods 50:1  pp. 250 ff. Crossref logo
Cox, Jessica G.
2019. Multilingualism in older age: A research agenda from the cognitive perspective. Language Teaching 52:3  pp. 360 ff. Crossref logo
Di Pisa, Grazia, Sergio Miguel Pereira Soares & Jason Rothman
2021. Brain, mind and linguistic processing insights into the dynamic nature of bilingualism and its outcome effects. Journal of Neurolinguistics 58  pp. 100965 ff. Crossref logo
Johannessen, Janne Bondi & Michael T. Putnam
2020.  In The Cambridge Handbook of Germanic Linguistics,  pp. 783 ff. Crossref logo
Ramos, Sara, Yuriem Fernández García, Eneko Antón, Aina Casaponsa & Jon Andoni Duñabeitia
2017. Does learning a language in the elderly enhance switching ability?. Journal of Neurolinguistics 43  pp. 39 ff. Crossref logo
van der Ploeg, Mara, Merel Keijzer & Wander Lowie
2020. Methodological concerns and their solutions in third-age language learning studies. Dutch Journal of Applied Linguistics 9:1-2  pp. 97 ff. Crossref logo
Zhu, Qiong-Bin, Ai-Min Bao & Dick Swaab
2019. Activation of the Brain to Postpone Dementia: A Concept Originating from Postmortem Human Brain Studies. Neuroscience Bulletin 35:2  pp. 253 ff. Crossref logo

This list is based on CrossRef data as of 25 may 2021. Please note that it may not be complete. Sources presented here have been supplied by the respective publishers. Any errors therein should be reported to them.

References

References

Abutalebi, J., Cappa, S.F., & Perani, D.
(2001) The bilingual brain as revealed by functional neuroimaging. Bilingualism: Language and Cognition, 4, 179–190. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Abutalebi, J., Della Rosa, P. A., Green, D. W., Hernandez, M., Scifo, P., Keim, R., Costa, A.
(2012) Bilingualism tunes the anterior cingulate cortex for conflict monitoring. Cerebral Cortex, 22(9), 2076–2086. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Adesope, O. O., Lavin, T., Thompson, T., & Ungerleider, C.
(2010) A systematic review and meta-analysis of the cognitive correlates of bilingualism. Review of Educational Research, 80, 207–245. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Alladi, S., Bak, T. H., Duggirala, V., Surampudi, B., Shailaja, M., Shukla, A. K., Kaul, S.
(2013) Bilingualism delays age at onset of dementia, independent of education and immigration status. Neurology, 81, 1938–1944. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Bak, T. H.
(2015) Beyond a simple “yes” and “no”: A Commentary on Paap et al. “Bilingual advantages in executive functioning either do not exist or are restricted to very specific and undetermined circumstances”. Cortex, 73, 332–333. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Barac, R., Bialystok, E., Castro, D. C., & Sanchez, M.
(2014) The cognitive development of young dual language learners: A critical review. Early Childhood Research Quarterly, 29, 699–714. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Bavelier, D., & Davidson, R. J.
(2013) Games to do you good. Nature, 494, 425–426. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Bialystok, E.
(2006) Effect of bilingualism and computer video game experience on the Simon task. Canadian Journal of Experimental Psychology-Revue Canadienne De Psychologie Experimentale, 60(1), 68–79. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
(2009a) Bilingualism: The good, the bad, and the indifferent. Bilingualism: Language and Cognition, 12(1), 3–11. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
(2009b) Claiming evidence from non-evidence: a reply to Morton and Harper. Developmental Science, 12(4), 499–501. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
(2015) Bilingualism and the development of executive function: The role of attention. Child Development Perspectives, 9(2), 117–121. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Bialystok, E., Martin, M.M., & Viswanathan, M.
(2005) Bilingualism across the lifespan: The rise and fall of inhibitory control. International Journal of Bilingualism, 9, 103–119. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Corbett, A., Owen, A., Hampshire, A., Grahn, J., Stenton, R., Dajani, S., Ballard, C.
(2015) The Effect of an Online Cognitive Training Package in Healthy Older Adults: An Online Randomized Controlled Trial. Journal of the American Medical Directors Association, 16(11), 990–997. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Costa, A., Hernandez, A., Costa-Faidella, J., & Sebastian-Galles, N.
(2009) On the bilingual advantage in conflict processing: Now you see it, now you don’t. Cognition, 113, 135–149. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Costa, A., & Sebastián-Gallés, N.
(2014) How does the bilingual experience sculpt the brain? Nature Reviews Neuroscience, 15(5), 336–345. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Draganski, B., Gaser, C., Busch, V., Schuierer, G., Bogdahn, U., & May, A.
(2004) Changes in grey matter induced by training. Nature, 427, 311–312. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Dunabeitia, J. A., Hernandez, J. A., Anton, E., Macizo, P., Estevez, A., Fuentes, L. J., & Carreiras, M.
(2014) The inhibitory advantage in bilingual children revisited: myth or reality? Experimental Psychology, 61(3), 234–251. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Ferreira, N., Owen, A., Mohan, A., Corbett, A., & Ballard, C.
(2015) Associations between cognitively stimulating leisure activities, cognitive function and age-related cognitive decline. International Journal of Geriatric Psychiatry, 30(4), 422–430. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Gathercole, V. C., Thomas, E. M., Kennedy, I., Prys, C., Young, N., Vinas Guasch, N., Jones, L.
(2014) Does language dominance affect cognitive performance in bilinguals? Lifespan evidence from preschoolers through older adults on card sorting, Simon, and metalinguistic tasks. Frontiers in Psychology, 5, 11. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Gold, B. T., Kim, C., Johnson, N. F., Kryscio, R. J., & Smith, C. D.
(2013) Lifelong bilingualism maintains neural efficiency for cognitive control in aging. Journal of Neuroscience, 33(2), 387–396. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Hetland, L.
(2000) Learning to make music enhances spatial reasoning. Journal of Aesthetic Education, 34, 179–238. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Kousaie, S., & Phillips, N. A.
(2012) Conflict monitoring and resolution: are two languages better than one? Evidence from reaction time and event-related brain potentials. Brain Research, 1446, 71–90. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Kovacs, A. M., & Mehler, J.
(2009) Cognitive gains in 7-month-old bilingual infants. Proceedings of the National Academy of Science, 106(16), 6556–6560. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Kroll, J. F., & Bialystok, E.
(2013) Understanding the consequences of bilingualism for language processing and cognition. Journal of Cognitive Psychology, 25(5), 497–514. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Kroll, J. F., & Dussias, P. E.
(2013) The comprehension of words and sentences in two languages. In T. B. W. Ritchie (Ed.), The Handbook of Bilingualism and Multilingualism, 2nd Edition (pp. 216–243). Malden, MA: Wiley-Blackwell Publishers.Google Scholar
Luk, G., Anderson, J. A. E., Craik, F. I. M., Grady, C., & Bialystok, E.
(2010) Distinct neural correlates for two types of inhibition in bilinguals: Response inhibition and intereference supression. Brain and Cognition, 74, 347–357. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Maguire, E. A., Gadian, D. G., Johnsrude, I. S., Good, C. D., Ashburner, J., Frackowiak, R. S. J., & Frith, C. D.
(2000) Navigation-related structural change in the hippocampi of taxi drivers. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, 97(8), 4398–4403. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Morton, J. P., & Harper, S. N.
(2007) What did Simon say? Revisiting the bilingual advantage. Developmental Science, 10, 719–726. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Owen, A. M., Hampshire, A., Grahn, J. A., Stenton, R., Dajani, S., Burns, A. S., . . . Ballard, C. G.
(2010) Putting brain training to the test. Nature, 465(7299), 775–778. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Paap, K.R., & Greenberg, Z.I.
(2013) There is no coherent evidence for a bilingual advantage in executive processing. Cognitive Psychology, 66, 232–258. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Paap, K. R., Johnson, H. A., & Sawi, O.
(2015) Bilingual advantages in executive functioning either do not exist or are restricted to very specific and undetermined circumstances. Cortex, 69, 265–278. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Pascual-Leone, A., Amedi, A., Fregni, F., & Merabet, L. B.
(2005) The plastic human brain cortex. Annual Review of Neuroscience, 28, 377–401. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Plassman, B. L., Langa, K. M., Fisher, G. G., Heeringa, S. G., Weir, D. R., Ofstedal, M. B., Wallace, R. B.
(2007) Prevalence of dementia in the United States: the aging, demographics, and memory study. Neuroepidemiology, 29(1-2), 125–132. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Prince, M., Bryce, R., Albanese, E., Wimo, A., Ribeiro, W., & Ferri, C. P.
(2013) The global prevalence of dementia: a systematic review and metaanalysis. Alzheimers & Dementia, 9(1), 63–75 e62. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Sebastian-Galles, N., Albareda-Castellot, B., Weikum, W. M., & Werker, J. F.
(2012) A bilingual advantage in visual language discrimination in infancy. Psychol Sci, 23(9), 994–999. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Stern, Y.
(2002) What is cognitive reserve? Theory and research application of the reserve concept. Journal of the International Neuropsychological Society, 8, 448–460. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
(2012) Cognitive reserve in ageing and Alzheimer’s disease. The Lancet Neurology, 11(11), 1006–1012. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Zahodne, L. B., Schofield, P. W., Farrell, M. T., Stern, Y., & Manly, J. J.
(2014) Bilingualism does not alter cognitive decline or dementia risk among Spanish-speaking immigrants. Neuropsychology, 28(2), 238–246. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Zhu, C. W., Livote, E. E., Scarmeas, N., Albert, M., Brandt, J., Blacker, D., Stern, Y.
(2013) Long-term associations between cholinesterase inhibitors and memantine use and health outcomes among patients with Alzheimer’s disease. Alzheimers & Dementia, 9, 63–75. CrossrefGoogle Scholar