Introduction published in:
Bilingualism and Executive Function: An interdisciplinary approach
Edited by Irina A. Sekerina and Lauren Spradlin
[Linguistic Approaches to Bilingualism 6:5] 2016
► pp. 505516
References

References

Bak, T. H.
(2016) Cooking pasta in La Paz: Bilingualism, bias and the replication crisis.Google Scholar
Bialystok, E.
(1999) Cognitive complexity and attentional control in the bilingual mind. Child Development, 70(3), 636–644. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
(2002) Acquisition of literacy in bilingual children: A framework for research. Language Learning, 52(1), 59–199. http://dx.doi.orgCrossref.Google Scholar
(2016) The signal and the noise: Finding the pattern in human behavior.Google Scholar
Bialystok, E., Craik, F. I. M., & Freedman, M.
(2007) Bilingualism as a protection against the onset of symptoms of dementia. Neuropsychologia, 45(2), 459–464. Crossref.Google Scholar
Bialystok, E., Craik, F. I. M., Klein, R., & Viswanathan, M.
(2004) Bilingualism, aging, and cognitive control: Evidence from the Simon task. Psychology and Aging, 19(2), 290–303. Crossref.Google Scholar
Bialystok, E., Craik, F. I. M., & Stefurak, T.
(2008) Planning and management in Parkinson’s disease: Differential emphasis in dual-task performance. Journal of the International Neuropsychological Society, 14(2), 257–265. Crossref.Google Scholar
Calvo, A., & Bialystok, E.
(2014) Independent effects of bilingualism and socioeconomic status on language ability and executive functioning. Cognition, 130(3), 278–288. Crossref.Google Scholar
Clahsen, H., & Veríssimo, J.
(2016) Investigating grammatical processing in bilinguals: The case of morphological priming.Google Scholar
Costa, A., Hernández, M., Costa-Faidella, J., & Sebastián-Gallés, N.
(2009) On the bilingual advantage in conflict processing: Now you see it, now you don’t. Cognition, 113(2), 135–149. Crossref.Google Scholar
Craik, F. I. M., Bialystok, E., & Freedman, M.
(2010) Delaying the onset of Alzheimer disease: Bilingualism as a form of cognitive reserve. Neurology, 75(19), 1726–1729. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
De Bruin, A., Treccani, B., & Della Sala, S.
(2015) Cognitive advantage in bilingualism: An example of publication bias? Psychological Science, 26(1), 99–107. Crossref.Google Scholar
De Groot, A. M. B., & Kroll, J. F.
(Eds.) (1997) Tutorials in Bilingualism: Psycholinguistic Perspectives. Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates. Crossref.Google Scholar
Dell’Amore, C.
(18 February 2011) To stave off Alzheimer’s, learn a language? National Geographic News. Retrieved from http://​news​.nationalgeographic​.com​/news​/2011​/02​/100218​-bilingual​-brains​-alzheimers​-dementia​-science​-aging/.
Donnelly, S., Brooks, P. J., & Homer, B.
(2015) Examining the bilingual advantage on conflict resolution tasks: A meta-analysis. In D. C. Noelle, R. Dale, A. S. Warlaumont, J. Yoshimi, T. Matlock, C. D. Jennings, & P. P. Maglio (Eds.), Proceedings of the 37th Annual Meeting of the Cognitive Science Society (pp. 596–601). Austin, TX: Cognitive Science Society.Google Scholar
Friedman, N. P.
(2016) Research on individual differences in executive functions: Implications for the Bilingual Advantage Hypothesis.Google Scholar
Gathercole, V. C. M., Thomas, E. M., Guasch, N. V., Kennedy, I., Prys, C., Young, N., Roberts, E. J., Hughes, E. K., & Jones, L.
(2016) Teasing apart factors influencing executive function performance in bilinguals and monolinguals at different ages.Google Scholar
Hilchey, M. D., & Klein, R. M.
(2011) Are there bilingual advantages on nonlinguistic interference tasks? Implications for the plasticity of executive control processes. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 18(4), 625–658. Crossref.Google Scholar
Hofweber, J., Jones, L., & Marinis, T.
(2016) Effects of dense code-switching on executive control.Google Scholar
Homel, P., Palij, M., & Aaronson, D.
(Eds.) (1987) Childhood Bilingualism: Aspects of Linguistic, Cognitive, and Social Development. Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Kinzler, K.
(11 March 2016) The superior social skills of bilinguals. New York Times Sunday Review. Retrieved from http://​www​.nytimes​.com​/2016​/03​/13​/opinion​/sunday​/the​-superior​-social​-skills​-of​-bilinguals​.html.
Klein, R.
(2016) What cognitive processes are likely to be exercised by bilingualism and does this exercise lead to extra-linguistic cognitive benefits?Google Scholar
Kostiuk, K.
(2016) Want a healthy lifestyle? Reap these benefits of being bilingual. FluentU blog. Retrieved from http://​www​.fluentu​.com​/blog​/benefits​-of​-being​-bilingual/.
Kroll, J. F., & Bialystok, E.
(2013) Understanding the consequences of bilingualism for language processing and cognition. Journal of Cognitive Psychology, 25(5), 497–514. Crossref.Google Scholar
Marton, K.
(2016) Executive control in bilingual children: Factors that influence the outcomesGoogle Scholar
Milgram, S.
(1974/2009) Obedience to Authority: An Experimental View. Harper Perennial Modern Classics. Crossref.Google Scholar
Miyake, A., Firedman, N. P., Emerson, M. J., Witzki, A. H., Howerter, A., & Wager, T. D.
(2000) The unity and diversity of executive functions and their contributions to complex "Frontal Lobe" tasks: a latent variable analysis. Cognitive Psychology, 41(1), 49–100. Crossref.Google Scholar
Morton, J. B., & Harper, S. N.
(2007) What did Simon say? Revisiting the bilingual advantage. Developmental Science, 10(6), 719–726. Crossref.Google Scholar
Olsen, R. K., Pangelinan, M. M., Bogulski, C., Chakravarty, M. M., Luk, G., Grady, C. L., & Bialystok, E.
(2015) The effect of lifelong bilingualism on regional grey and white matter volume. Brain Research, 1612, 128–139. Crossref.Google Scholar
Paap, K. R.
(2014) The role of componential analysis, categorical hypothesing, replicability and confirmation bias in testing for bilingual advantages in executive functioning. Journal of Cognitive Psychology, 26 (3), 242–255. Crossref.Google Scholar
Paap, K. R., & Greenberg, Z. I.
(2013) There is no coherent evidence for a bilingual advantage in executive processing. Cognitive Psychology, 66(2), 232–258. Crossref.Google Scholar
Paap, K. R. Johnson, H. A., & Sawi, O.
(2015) Bilingual advantages in executive functioning either do not exist or are restricted to very specific and undetermined circumstances. Cortex, 69, 265–278. Crossref.Google Scholar
Pashler, H. & Harris, C. R.
(2012) Is the replicability crisis overblown? Three arguments examined. Perspectives on Psychological Science, 7(6), 531–536. Crossref.Google Scholar
Prior, A., & Gollan, T. H.
(2011) Good language-switchers are good task-switchers: Evidence from Spanish-English and Mandarin-English bilinguals. Journal of the International Neuropsychological Society, 17(4), 682–691. Crossref.Google Scholar
Simons, D. J.
(2014) The value of direct replication. Perspectives on Psychological Science, 9(1), 76–80. Crossref.Google Scholar
Sorace, A.
(2016) Referring expressions and executive functions in bilingualism.Google Scholar
Sorge, G. B., Toplak, M. E., & Bialystok, E.
in press). Interactions between levels of attention ability and levels of bilingualism in children's executive functioning. Developmental Science. Crossref.
Soveri, A., Rodriguez-Fornells, A., & Laine, M.
(2011) Is there a relationship between language switching and executive functions in bilingualism? Introducing a within-group analysis approach. Frontiers in Psychology, 2. ArtID: 183 Crossref.Google Scholar
Stroebe, W., & Strack, F.
(2014) The alleged crisis and the illusion of exact replication. Perspectives on Psychological Science, 9(1), 59–71. Crossref.Google Scholar
Valian, V. V.
(2015) Bilingualism and cognition. Bilingualism: Language and Cognition, 18(1), 3–24. Crossref.Google Scholar
(2016) Putting together bilingualism and executive function.Google Scholar
Watson, C. W.-M., Manly, J. J., & Zahodne, L. B.
(2016) Does bilingualism protect against cognitive aging? Methodological issues in research on bilingualism, cognitive reserve, and dementia incidence.Google Scholar
Yow, W. Q., & Li, X.
(2015) Balanced bilingualism and early age of second language acquisition as the underlying mechanisms of a bilingual executive control advantage: why variations in bilingual experiences matter. Frontiers in psychology, 6. Crossref.Google Scholar