Article published In:
Linguistic Approaches to Bilingualism
Vol. 9:2 (2019) ► pp.314340
References (48)
References
Antoniou, K., & Katsos, N. (2017). The effect of childhood multilingualism and bilectalism on implicatures understanding. Applied Psycholinguistics, 1–47. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Barner, D., Brooks, N., & Bale, A. (2011). Accessing the unsaid: The role of scalar alternatives in children’s pragmatic inference. Cognition, 1181, 84–93. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Bates, D., Maechler, M., Bolker, B., & Walker, S. (2015). lme4: Linear mixed-effects models using Eigen and S4. R package version 1.1–8. Retrieved from [URL].
Bialystok, E. (1988). Levels of bilingualism and levels of linguistic awareness. Developmental Psychology, 241, 560–567. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
(1993). Metalinguistic awareness: “The development of children’s representations of language”. In C. Pratt, A. Garton (eds), Systems of representation in children: development and use (pp. 211–233). London: Wiley & Sons.Google Scholar
(1999). Cognitive complexity and attentional control in the bilingual mind. Child Development, 701, 636–644. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
2010. Bilingualism. Wiley interdisciplinary reviews: Cognitive science, 11, 559–572.Google Scholar
(2011). Reshaping the mind: The benefits of bilingualism. Canadian Journal of Experimental Psychology, 651, 229–235. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Bialystok, E., Craik, F., Green, D. & Gollan, T. (2009). Bilingual minds. Psychological Science in the Public Interest, 101, 89–129. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Bialystok, E., Craik, F. & Luk, G. (2012). Bilingualism: consequences for the mind and brain. Trends in Cognitive Science, 16/4, 240–250. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Bialystok, E., Luk, G., Peets, K. F., & Yang, S. (2010). Receptive vocabulary differences in monolingual and bilingual children. Bilingualism: Language and Cognition, 131, 525–531. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Bott, L., Bailey, T. M., & Grodner, D. (2012). Distinguishing speed from accuracy in scalar implicatures. Journal of Memory and Language, 661, 123–142. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Bott, L., & Noveck, I. A. (2004). Some utterances are underinformative: The onset and time course of scalar inferences. Journal of Memory and Language, 511, 437–457. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Braine, M. D. S., & Rumain, B. (1981). Development of comprehension of “Or”: Evidence for a sequence of competencies. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 311, 46–70. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Chemla, E., & Spector, B. (2011). Experimental evidence for embedded scalar implicatures. Journal of Semantics, 281, 359–400. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Costa, A., & Sebastián-Gallés, N. (2014). How does the bilingual experience sculpt the brain? Nature Reviews Neuroscience, 151, 336–345. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Cromdal, J. (1999). Childhood bilingualism and metalinguistic skills: Analysis and control in young Swedish-English bilinguals. Applied Psycholinguistics, 201, 1–20. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Degen, J., & Tanenhaus, M. K. (2011) Making inferences: the case of scalar implicatures processing. In L. Carlson, C. Holscher, and T. Shipley (Eds.), Proceedings of the 33rd Annual Conference of the Cognitive Science Society (pp. 3299–3304). Austin, TX: Cognitive Science Society.Google Scholar
Dunn, L. M., & Dunn, L. M. (1981). Peabody Picture Vocabulary Test. Circle Pines: American Guidance Service.Google Scholar
Feeney, A., Scrafton, S., Duckworth, A., & Handley, S. J. (2004). The story of some: Everyday pragmatic inference by children and adults. Canadian Journal of Experimental Psychology, 58/2, 121–132. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Fernández, E. M. (2003). Bilingual Sentence Processing: Relative clause attachment in English and Spanish, In Language Acquisition and Language Disorders, Vol.291, Amsterdam: John Benjamins Publishing Company. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Foppolo, F., & Guasti, M. T. (2012). Scalar implicatures in child language: give children a chance. Language Learning and Development, 81, 365–394. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Genesee, F., Nicoladis, E., & Paradis, J. (1995). Language differentiation in early bilingual development. Journal of Child Language, 221, 611–631. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Grice, P. (1989). Studies in the Way of Words. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.Google Scholar
Grodner, D. J., Klein, N. M., Carbary, K. M., and Tanenhaus, M. K. (2010). “Some,” and possibly all, scalar inferences are not delayed: Evidence for immediate pragmatic enrichment. Cognition, 1161, 42–55. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Gualmini, A., Crain, S., Meroni, L., Chierchia, G., & Guasti, M. T. (2001). At the semantics/pragmatics interface in child language. In R. Hastings, B. Jackson, and Z. Zvolensky (eds.) Semantics and Linguistic Theory (SALT XI), (231–247). Ithaca, NY: Cornell University.Google Scholar
Guasti, M. T., Chierchia, G., Crain, S., Foppolo, F., Gualmini, A., & Meroni, L. (2005). Why children and adults sometimes (but not always) compute implicatures. Language and Cognitive Processes, 20:5, 667–696. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Hilbe, J. M. (2009). Logistic Regression Models. Boca Raton: Chapman and Hall, CRC Texts in Statistical Science. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Horn, L. R. (2004). Implicature. In L. R. Horn, & G. Ward (Eds.), The Handbook of Pragmatics (3–28). Oxford: Blackwell Publishing.Google Scholar
Hothorn, T., Bretz, F., & Westfall, P. (2008). Simultaneous Inference in General Parametric Models. Biometrical Journal, 50/3, 346–363. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Huang, Y. T., & Snedeker, J. (2009). Online interpretation of scalar quantifiers: Insight into the semantics-pragmatics interface. Cognitive Psychology, 581, 376–415. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Katsos, N., & Bishop, D. V. M. (2011). Pragmatic tolerance: Implications for the acquisition of informativeness and implicatures. Cognition, 1201, 67–81. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Levinson, S. C. (2000). Presumptive Meanings: The Theory of Generalized Conversational Implicature. Language, Speech, and Communication. Cambridge, MA: The MIT Press. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Noveck, I. A. (2001). When children are more logical than adults: Experimental investigations of scalar implicatures. Cognition, 781, 165–188. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Noveck, I. A., & Posada, A. (2003). Characterizing the time course of an implicature. Brain and Language, 851, 203–210. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Noveck, I. A., & Sperber, D. (2007). The why and how of experimental pragmatics: The case of ‘scalar inferences’. In N. Burton-Roberts (Ed.), Advances in Pragmatics (pp. 184–212). Basingstoke: Palgrave. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Oller, D., Pearson, B., & Cobo-Lewis, A. (2007). Profile effects in early bilingual language and literacy. Applied Psycholinguistics, 281, 191–230. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Papafragou, A., & Musolino, J. (2003). Scalar implicatures: Experiments at the semantics-pragmatics interface. Cognition, 861, 253–282. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Papafragou, A., & Tantalou, N. (2004). Children’s computation of implicatures. Language Acquisition, 12/1, 71–82. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Pouscoulous, N., Noveck, I. A., Politzer, G., & Bastide, A. (2007). A developmental investigation of processing costs in implicature production. Language Acquisition, 14/4, 347–375. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Siegal, M., Matsuo, A., Pond, C., & Otsu, Y. (2007). Bilingualism and cognitive development: Evidence from scalar implicatures. In Y. Otsu (Ed.), Proceedings from the Eight Tokyo Conference on Psycholinguistics. Tokyo: Hituzi Syobo.Google Scholar
Siegal, M., Iozzi, L., & Surian, L. (2009). Bilingualism and conversational understanding in young children. Cognition, 1101, 115–122. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Siegal, M., Surian, L., Matsuo, A., Geraci, A., Iozzi, L., Okumura, Y., & Itakura, S. (2010). Bilingualism accentuates children’s conversational understanding. PloS One, 5:2, e9004. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Skordos, D., & Papafragou, A. (2014). Scalar inferences in 5-year-olds: the role of alternatives. In Proceedings of the 38th Annual Boston University Conference on Language Development. Somerville, MA: Cascadilla Press.Google Scholar
Slabakova, R. (2010) Scalar implicatures in second language acquisition. Lingua, 1201, 2444–2462. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Sperber, D., & Wilson, D. (1995). Relevance: Communication and Cognition. Oxford: Basil Blackwell.Google Scholar
Stateva, P., Déprez, V., Dupuy, L., Reboul, A., & Stepanov, A. (in preparation) Proportional modifiers and semantic universals.
Stella, G., Pizzoli, C., & Tressoldi, P. E. (2000). PPVT-R, Peabody Picture Vocabulary Test-Revised. Torino: Omega Edizioni.Google Scholar
Cited by (12)

Cited by 12 other publications

Geiss, Miriam, Maria F Ferin, Theo Marinis & Tanja Kupisch
2024. Rhetorical question comprehension by Italian–German bilingual children. Second Language Research 40:2  pp. 221 ff. DOI logo
Nehls, Paul N., Kodai Aramaki & Tomohiro Fujii
2023. Is a quantifier mismatch a problem for L1 Japanese learners of English?. Linguistics Beyond and Within (LingBaW) 9  pp. 133 ff. DOI logo
Cho, Jacee
2022. Crosslinguistic influence on L2 implicature computation for determiners. Journal of Second Language Studies 5:1  pp. 86 ff. DOI logo
Feng, Shuo
2022. The computation and suspension of presuppositions by L1-Mandarin Chinese L2-English speakers. Second Language Research 38:4  pp. 737 ff. DOI logo
Khorsheed, Ahmed, Sabariah Md. Rashid, Vahid Nimehchisalem, Lee Geok Imm, Jessica Price, Camilo R. Ronderos & Claudia Felser
2022. What second-language speakers can tell us about pragmatic processing. PLOS ONE 17:2  pp. e0263724 ff. DOI logo
Starr, Glenn & Jacee Cho
2022. QUD sensitivity in the computation of scalar implicatures in second language acquisition. Language Acquisition 29:2  pp. 182 ff. DOI logo
ANTONIOU, KYRIAKOS & EMMANOUELA MILAKI
2021. Irony Comprehension in Bidialectal Speakers. The Modern Language Journal 105:3  pp. 697 ff. DOI logo
Mazzaggio, Greta, Daniele Panizza & Luca Surian
2021. On the interpretation of scalar implicatures in first and second language. Journal of Pragmatics 171  pp. 62 ff. DOI logo
Stateva, Penka, Sara Andreetta, Anne Reboul & Arthur Stepanov
2021. Bilingual Children’s Use of the ‘Maximize Presupposition’ Principle. Glossa: a journal of general linguistics 6:1 DOI logo
Kecskes, Istvan
2019. English as a Lingua Franca, DOI logo
Fekete, István, Petra Schulz & Esther Ruigendijk
2018. Exhaustivity in single bare <i>wh</i>-questions: A differential-analysis of exhaustivity. Glossa: a journal of general linguistics 3:1 DOI logo
Snape, Neal & Hironobu Hosoi
2018. Acquisition of scalar implicatures. Linguistic Approaches to Bilingualism 8:2  pp. 163 ff. DOI logo

This list is based on CrossRef data as of 5 july 2024. Please note that it may not be complete. Sources presented here have been supplied by the respective publishers. Any errors therein should be reported to them.