Article published in:
Psycholinguistic approaches to production and comprehension in bilingual adults and children
Edited by Maialen Iraola Azpiroz, Shanley E.M. Allen, Kalliopi Katsika and Leigh Fernandez
[Linguistic Approaches to Bilingualism 9:4/5] 2019
► pp. 542568
References

References

Baayen, R. H., Piepenbrock, R., & Van Rijn, H.
(1993) The CELEX lexical database (CD-ROM). Philadelphia: University of Pennsylvania, Linguistic Data Consortium.Google Scholar
Bak, T. H.
(2016) Cooking pasta in La Paz Bilingualism, bias and the replication crisis. Linguistic Approaches to Bilingualism, 6(5), 699–717. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Benjamini, Y., & Hochberg, Y.
(1995) Controlling the false discovery rate – A practical and powerful approach to multiple testing. Journal of the Royal Statistical Society Series B-Methodological, 57(1), 289–300. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Bialystok, E., Craik, F. I. M., Klein, R., & Viswanathan, M.
(2004) Bilingualism, aging, and cognitive control: Evidence from the Simon task. Psychology and Aging, 19(2), 290–303. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Branzi, F. M., Calabria, M., Boscarino, M. L., & Costa, A.
(2016) On the overlap between bilingual language control and domain-general executive control. Acta Psychologica, 166, 21–30. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Calabria, M., Branzi, F. M., Marne, P., Hernandez, M., & Costa, A.
(2015) Age-related effects over bilingual language control and executive control. Bilingualism-Language and Cognition, 18(1), 65–78. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Calabria, M., Hernandez, M., Branzi, F. M., & Costa, A.
(2012) Qualitative differences between bilingual language control and executive control: evidence from task-switching. Frontiers in Psychology, 3.Google Scholar
Christoffels, I. K., Firk, C., & Schiller, N. O.
(2007) Bilingual language control: An event-related brain potential study. Brain Research, 1147, 192–208. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Cook, S. V., & Gor, K.
(2015) Lexical access in L2 Representational deficit or processing constraint? Mental Lexicon, 10(2), 247–270. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Costa, A., Hernandez, M., Costa-Faidella, J., & Sebastian-Galles, N.
(2009) On the bilingual advantage in conflict processing: Now you see it, now you don’t. Cognition, 113(2), 135–149. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Costa, A., Hernandez, M., & Sebastian-Galles, N.
(2008) Bilingualism aids conflict resolution: Evidence from the ANT task. Cognition, 106(1), 59–86. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Costa, A., & Santesteban, M.
(2004) Lexical access in bilingual speech production: Evidence from language switching in highly proficient bilinguals and L2 learners. Journal of Memory and Language, 50(4), 491–511. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Dijkstra, T., Van Heuven, W. J. B., & Grainger, J.
(1998) Simulating cross-language competition with the bilingual interactive activation model. Psychologica Belgica, 38(3–4), 177–196.Google Scholar
Dimitropoulou, M., Dunabeitia, J. A., & Carreiras, M.
(2011) Two words, one meaning: evidence of automatic co-activation of translation equivalents. Frontiers in Psychology, 2. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Festman, J., Rodriguez-Fornells, A., & Munte, T. F.
(2010) Individual differences in control of language interference in late bilinguals are mainly related to general executive abilities. Behavioral and Brain Functions, 6. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Fink, A., & Goldrick, M.
(2015) Pervasive benefits of preparation in language switching. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 22(3), 808–814. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Friedman, N. P.
(2016) Research on individual differences in executive functions Implications for the bilingual advantage hypothesis. Linguistic Approaches to Bilingualism, 6(5), 535–548. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Gambi, C., & Hartsuiker, R. J.
(2016) If you stay, it might be easier: Switch costs from comprehension to production in a joint switching task. Journal of Experimental Psychology-Learning Memory and Cognition, 42(4), 608–626. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Gollan, T. H., & Ferreira, V. S.
(2009) Should I stay or should I switch? A cost-benefit analysis of voluntary language switching in young and aging bilinguals. Journal of Experimental Psychology-Learning Memory and Cognition, 35(3), 640–665. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Gollan, T. H., Kleinman, D., & Wierenga, C. E.
(2014) What’s easier: Doing what you want, or being told what to do? Cued versus voluntary language and task switching. Journal of Experimental Psychology-General, 143(6), 2167–2195. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Gollan, T. H., Montoya, R. I., & Werner, G. A.
(2002) Semantic and letter fluency in Spanish-English bilinguals. Neuropsychology, 16(4), 562–576. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Green, D. W.
(1998) Mental control of the bilingual lexico-semantic system. Bilingualism: Language and Cognition, 1(2), 67–81. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Hernandez, A. E., & Kohnert, K. J.
(2015) Investigations into the locus of language-switching costs in older adult bilinguals. Bilingualism-Language and Cognition, 18(1), 51–64. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Hernandez, A. E., Martinez, A., & Kohnert, K.
(2000) In search of the language switch: An fMRI study of picture naming in Spanish-English bilinguals. Brain and Language, 73(3), 421–431. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Hilchey, M. D., & Klein, R. M.
(2011) Are there bilingual advantages on nonlinguistic interference tasks? Implications for the plasticity of executive control processes. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 18(4), 625–658. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Hoshino, N., & Thierry, G.
(2011) Language selection in bilingual word production: Electrophysiological evidence for cross-language competition. Brain Research, 1371, 100–109. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Hugdahl, K., Lundervold, A., Ersland, L., Smievoll, A. I., Sundberg, H., Barndon, R., & Roscher, B. E.
(1999) Left frontal activation during a semantic categorization task: An fMRI-study. International Journal of Neuroscience, 99(1–4), 49–58. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Kaan, E., Harris, A., Gibson, E., & Holcomb, P.
(2000) The P600 as an index of syntactic integration difficulty. Language and Cognitive Processes, 15(2), 159–201. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Kolk, H. H. J., Chwilla, D. J., van Herten, M., & Oor, P. J. W.
(2003) Structure and limited capacity in verbal working memory: A study with event-related potentials. Brain and Language, 85(1), 1–36. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Le Grand Robert Langue française
(2017) Retrieved 2017, September 28 http://​grand​-robert​.lerobert​.com/
Levelt, W. J. M.
(1983) Monitoring and self-repair in speech. Cognition, 14(1), 41–104. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Libben, M. R., & Titone, D. A.
(2009) Bilingual lexical access in context: Evidence from eye movements during reading. Journal of Experimental Psychology-Learning Memory and Cognition, 35(2), 381–390. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Linck, J. A., Schwieter, J. W., & Sunderman, G.
(2012) Inhibitory control predicts language switching performance in trilingual speech production. Bilingualism-Language and Cognition, 15(3), 651–662. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Ma, F. Y., Li, S. C., & Guo, T. M.
(2016) Reactive and proactive control in bilingual word production: An investigation of influential factors. Journal of Memory and Language, 86, 35–59. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Macizo, P., Bajo, T., & Paolieri, D.
(2012) Language switching and language competition. Second Language Research, 28(2), 131–149. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Marian, V., Blumenfeld, H. K., & Kaushanskaya, M.
(2007) The Language Experience and Proficiency Questionnaire (LEAP-Q): Assessing language profiles in bilinguals and multilinguals. Journal of Speech Language and Hearing Research, 50(4), 940–967. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Martin, C. D., Strijkers, K., Santesteban, M., Escera, C., Hartsuiker, R. J., & Costa, A.
(2013) The impact of early bilingualism on controlling a language learned late: an ERP study. Frontiers in Psychology, 4. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Marton, K.
(2016) Executive control in bilingual children Factors that influence the outcomes. Linguistic Approaches to Bilingualism, 6(5), 575–589. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Meuter, R. F. I., & Allport, A.
(1999) Bilingual language switching in naming: Asymmetrical costs of language selection. Journal of Memory and Language, 40(1), 25–40. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Orfanidou, E., & Sumner, P.
(2005) Language switching and the effects of orthographic specificity and response repetition. Memory & Cognition, 33(2), 355–369. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Prior, A., Degani, T., Awawdy, S., Yassin, R., & Korem, N.
(2017) Is susceptibility to cross-language interference domain specific? Cognition, 165, 10–25. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Prior, A., & Gollan, T. H.
(2011) Good Language-Switchers are Good Task-Switchers: Evidence from Spanish-English and Mandarin-English Bilinguals. Journal of the International Neuropsychological Society, 17(4), 682–691. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Reynolds, M. G., Schloffel, S., & Peressotti, F.
(2016) Asymmetric switch costs in numeral naming and number word reading: Implications for models of bilingual language production. Frontiers in Psychology, 6. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Roelofs, A., Piai, V., Rodriguez, G. G., & Chwilla, D. J.
(2016) Electrophysiology of cross-language interference and facilitation in picture naming. Cortex, 76, 1–16. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Sauval, K., Perre, L., Duncan, L. G., Marinus, E., & Casalis, S.
(2017) Automatic phonological activation during visual word recognition in bilingual children: A cross-language masked priming study in grades 3 and 5. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 154, 64–77. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Schwartz, A. I., & Kroll, J. F.
(2006) Bilingual lexical activation in sentence context. Journal of Memory and Language, 55(2), 197–212. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Singh, N., & Mishra, R. K.
(2015) The modulatory role of second language proficiency on performance monitoring: evidence from a saccadic countermanding task in high and low proficient bilinguals. Frontiers in Psychology, 5. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Soveri, A., Rodriguez-Fornells, A., & Laine, M.
(2011) Is there a relationship between language switching and executive functions in bilingualism? Introducing a within-group analysis approach. Frontiers in Psychology, 2. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Teubner-Rhodes, S. E., Mishler, A., Corbett, R., Andreu, L., Sanz-Torrent, M., Trueswell, J. C., & Novick, J. M.
(2016) The effects of bilingualism on conflict monitoring, cognitive control, and garden-path recovery. Cognition, 150, 213–231. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Thomas, M. S. C., & Allport, A.
(2000) Language switching costs in bilingual visual word recognition. Journal of Memory and Language, 43(1), 44–66. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Tse, C. S., & Altarriba, J.
(2015) Local and global task switching costs in bilinguals who vary in second language proficiency. American Journal of Psychology, 128(1), 89–106. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Van Dale Online Professioneel
(2017) Retrieved 2017, September 28 http://​www​.vandale​.nl​/opzoeken
van de Meerendonk, N., Chwilla, D. J., & Kolk, H. H. J.
(2013) States of indecision in the brain: ERP reflections of syntactic agreement violations versus visual degradation. Neuropsychologia, 51(8), 1383–1396. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
van de Meerendonk, N., Indefrey, P., Chwilla, D. J., & Kolk, H. H. J.
(2011) Monitoring in language perception: Electrophysiological and hemodynamic responses to spelling violations. Neuroimage, 54(3), 2350–2363. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
van de Meerendonk, N., Kolk, H. H. J., Vissers, C., & Chwilla, D. J.
(2010) Monitoring in language perception: Mild and strong conflicts elicit different ERP patterns. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 22(1), 67–82. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
van Hell, J. G., & Tanner, D.
(2012) Second language proficiency and cross-language lexical activation. Language Learning, 62, 148–171. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Vissers, C., Kolk, H. H. J., van de Meerendonk, N., & Chwilla, D. J.
(2008) Monitoring in language perception: Evidence from ERPs in a picture-sentence matching task. Neuropsychologia, 46(4), 967–982. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
von Bastian, C. C., Souza, A. S., & Gade, M.
(2016) No evidence for bilingual cognitive advantages: A test of four hypotheses. Journal of Experimental Psychology-General, 145(2), 246–258. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Wang, X.
(2015) Language control in bilingual language comprehension: evidence from the maze task. Frontiers in Psychology, 6. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Wang, Y. P., Kuhl, P. K., Chen, C. H., & Dong, Q.
(2009) Sustained and transient language control in the bilingual brain. Neuroimage, 47(1), 414–422. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Watson, C. W. M., Manly, J. J., & Zahodne, L. B.
(2016) Does bilingualism protect against cognitive aging? Methodological issues in research on bilingualism, cognitive reserve, and dementia incidence. Linguistic Approaches to Bilingualism, 6(5), 590–604. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Wiseheart, M., Viswanathan, M., & Bialystok, E.
(2016) Flexibility in task switching by monolinguals and bilinguals. Bilingualism-Language and Cognition, 19(1), 141–146. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Woumans, E., Ceuleers, E., Van der Linden, L., Szmalec, A., & Duyck, W.
(2015) Verbal and nonverbal cognitive control in bilinguals and interpreters. Journal of Experimental Psychology-Learning Memory and Cognition, 41(5), 1579–1586. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Yang, S. J., & Yang, H. J.
(2016) Bilingual effects on deployment of the attention system in linguistically and culturally homogeneous children and adults. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 146, 121–136. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Cited by

Cited by other publications

Azpiroz, Maialen Iraola, Shanley E. M. Allen, Kalliopi Katsika & Leigh Fernandez
2019. Psycholinguistic approaches to production and comprehension in bilingual adults and children. Linguistic Approaches to Bilingualism 9:4-5  pp. 505 ff. Crossref logo

This list is based on CrossRef data as of 08 january 2021. Please note that it may not be complete. Sources presented here have been supplied by the respective publishers. Any errors therein should be reported to them.