Article published in:
Linguistic Approaches to Bilingualism
Vol. 9:3 (2019) ► pp. 427467
References

References

Ackema, P., & Neeleman, A.
(1998) WHOT? In P. Barbosa & A. Et (Eds.), Is the Best Good Enough (pp. 15–34). Cambridge: MIT Press.Google Scholar
Argyri, E., & Sorace, A.
(2007) Crosslinguistic influence and language dominance in older bilingual children. Bilingualism: Language and Cognition, 10(1), 79. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Auer, P.
(1999) From codeswitching via language mixing to fused lects: Toward a dynamic typology of bilingual speech. International Journal of Bilingualism, 3(4), 309–332. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Austin, J., Blume, M., & Sánchez, L.
(2013) Syntactic Development in the L1 of Spanish-English Bilingual Children. Hispania, 96(3), 542–561. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Baayen, H.
(2008) Analyzing Linguistic Data a Practical Introduction to Statistics using R. Leiden: Cambridge University Press CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Baković, E.
(1998) Optimality and Inversion in Spanish. Is the Best Good Enough?: Optimality and Competition in Syntax, 35–58.Google Scholar
Bakovic, E., & Keer, E.
(2001) Optionality and ineffability. In G. Legendre, J. Grimshaw, & S. Vikner (Eds.), Optimality-theoretic syntax (pp. 97–112). Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.Google Scholar
Belazi, H. M., Rubin, E. J., & Toribio, A. J.
(1994) Code switching and X-bar theory: The functional head constraint. Linguistic Inquiry, 221–237.Google Scholar
Bernardini, P., & Schlyter, S.
(2004) Growing syntactic structure and code-mixing in the weaker language: The Ivy Hypothesis. Bilingualism: Language and Cognition, 7(1), 49–69. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Bishop, D. V.
(1989) Test for the reception of grammar (TROG). Medical Research Council.Google Scholar
Bonnesen, M.
(2005) Der erwerb der linken Satzperipherie bei Französisch/Deutsch bilingual aufwachsenden Kindern. Unpublished dissertation (Hamburg University).Google Scholar
Boser, K., Lust, B., Santelmann, L., Whitman, J., & Broderick
(1992) The syntax of CP and V-2 in early child German: The Strong Continuity Hypothesis. In Proceedings of NELS (Vol. 22, pp. 51–65).Google Scholar
Brown, E. L., & Rivas, J.
(2011) Subject-verb word order in Spanish interrogatives: A quantitative analysis of Puerto Rican Spanish. Spanish in Context, 8(1), 23–49. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Camacho, J.
(2006) In situ focus in Caribbean Spanish: Towards a unified account of focus. In Selected Proceedings of the 9th Hispanic Linguistics Symposium. Somerville: Cascadilla Proceedings Project (pp. 13–23).Google Scholar
Chomsky, N.
(1969) Aspects of the Theory of Syntax (Vol. 119). MIT press.Google Scholar
(1993) A minimalist program for linguistic theory. In K. Hale and S. J. Keyser (eds.), The View from Building 20: Essays in Linguistics in Honor of Sylvain Bromberger. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press. 1–52.Google Scholar
Coetzee, A. W.
(2009) Learning lexical indexation. Phonology, 26(01), 109–145. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Contreras, H.
(1987) Small Clauses in Spanish and English. Natural Language & Linguistic Theory, 5(2), 225–243. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Cuza, A.
(2016) The status of interrogative subject-verb inversion in Spanish-English bilingual children. Lingua. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Dale, P. S., & Fenson, L.
(1996) Lexical development norms for young children. Behavior Research Methods, Instruments, & Computers, 28(1), 125–127. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Davidson, L., & Legendre, G.
(2003) Defaults and competition in the acquisition of functional categories in Catalan and French. In R. Núñez-Cedeño, R. Cameron, & L. López (Eds.), Romance Perspective on Language Knowledge and Use: Selected Papers from the 31st Linguistic Symposium on Romance Languages (LSRL), Chicago, 19–22 April 2001 (pp. 273–286). Amsterdam: John Benjamins Publishing Company.Google Scholar
de Villiers, J.
(1991) Why questions. In T. L. Maxfield & B. Plunkett (Eds.), Papers in the Acquisition of WH: Proceedings of the UMass Roundtable, May 1990 (pp. 155–173). Amherst, MA: University of Massachusetts Occasional Papers in Linguistics.Google Scholar
Döpke, S.
(1998) Competing language structures: The acquisition of verb placement by bilingual German-English children. Journal of Child Language, 25(3), 555–584. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
(2000) Generation of and retraction from cross-linguistically motivated structures in bilingual first language acquisition. Bilingualism: Language and Cognition, 3(3), 209–226.Google Scholar
Dunn, L. M., & Dunn, D. M.
(2007) PPVT-4: Peabody picture vocabulary test. Minneapolis, MN: Pearson Assessments.Google Scholar
Dunn, L. M., Padilla, E. R., Lugo, D. E., & Dunn, L. M.
(1986) Test de Vocabulario en Images Peabody (Peabody Picture Vocabulary Test) Adaptacion Hispanoamericana (Hispanic American adaptation). Circle Pines, MN: American Guidance Service.Google Scholar
Fernández-Fuertes, R., & Liceras, J. M.
(2010) Copula omission in the English developing grammar of English/Spanish bilingual children. International Journal of Bilingual Education and Bilingualism, 13(5), 525–551. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Fikkert, P.
(2007) Acquiring phonology. Handbook of Phonological Theory, 537–554.Google Scholar
Foroodi-Nejad, F., & Paradis, J.
(2009) Crosslinguistic transfer in the acquisition of compound words in Persian-English bilinguals. Bilingualism: Language and Cognition, 12(4), 411–427. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Fukazawa, H., Kitahara, M., & Ota, M.
(1998) Lexical stratification and ranking invariance in constraint-based grammars. In Proceedings of Chicago Linguistics Society (Vol. 34, pp. 47–62).Google Scholar
Gawlitzek-Maiwald, I., & Tracy, R.
(1996) Bilingual bootstrapping. Linguistics, 34(5), 901–296. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Goodall, G.
(1991) On the Status of Spec of IP. In The Proceedings of the Tenth West Coast Conference on Formal Linguistics (pp. 175–182).Google Scholar
(1993) Spec of IP and Spec of CP in Spanish Wh-questions. Amsterdam Studies in the Theory and History of Linguistic Science Series 4, 199–199.Google Scholar
(2004) On the syntax and processing of wh-questions in Spanish. In Proceedings of the 23rd West Coast Conference on Formal Linguistics (pp. 101–114). Citeseer.Google Scholar
Grimshaw, J.
(1997) Projection, Heads, and Optimality. Linguistic Inquiry, 28(3), 373–422.Google Scholar
Groos, A., Bok-Bennema, R., Bordelois, I., Contreras, H., & Zagona, K.
(1986) The structure of a sentence in Spanish. In Generative studies in Spanish syntax (pp. 67–80).Google Scholar
Grosjean, F.
(1989) Neurolinguists, beware! The bilingual is not two monolinguals in one person. Brain and Language, 36(1), 3–15. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Guasti, M. T.
(1996) On the controversial status of Romance interrogatives. Probus, 8, 161–180. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Guilfoyle, E., & Noonan, M.
(1992) Functional categories and language-acquisition. Canadian Journal of Linguistics-Revue Canadienne de Linguistique, 37(2), 241–272. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Hartsuiker, R. J., Pickering, M. J., & Veltkamp, E.
(2004) Is Syntax Separate or Shared Between Languages? Psychological Science, 15(3), 409–414. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Hayes, B.
(2004) Phonological acquisition in Optimality Theory: the early stages. Constraints in Phonological Acquisition, 158–203.Google Scholar
Hendriks, P.
(2016) Cognitive Modeling of Individual Variation in Reference Production and Comprehension. Language Sciences, 506. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Hendriks, P., & Spenader, J.
(2006) When production precedes comprehension: An optimization approach to the acquisition of pronouns. Language Acquisition, 13(4), 319–348. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Hernández-Pina, F.
(1984) Teorías psicosociolingüísticas y su aplicación del español como lengua materna [Psycholinguistic theories and their application to the acquisition of Spanish as a native language]. Siglo XXI, Madrid.Google Scholar
Hsin, L.
(2014) English questions, Spanish structure. In Variation within and across Romance Languages: Selected papers from the 41st Linguistic Symposium on Romance Languages (LSRL), Ottawa, 5–7 May 2011 (Vol. 333, p. 379). John Benjamins Publishing Company.Google Scholar
Hsin, L., Legendre, G., & Omaki, A.
(2013) Priming Cross-Linguistic Interference in Spanish-English Bilingual Children. In S. Baiz, N. Goldman, & R. Hawkes (Eds.), Proceedings of the 37th Boston University Conference on Language Development. (pp. 165–177). Somerville, MA: Cascadilla Proceedings Project.Google Scholar
Hulk, A., & Müller, N.
(2000) Bilingual first language acquisition at the interface between syntax and pragmatics. Bilingualism: Language and Cognition, 3(3), 227–244. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Hyams, N.
(1992) Morphosyntactic development in Italian and its relevance to parameter-setting models: comments on the paper by Pizzuto & Caselli. Journal of Child Language, 19(3), 695–709. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Itô, J., & Mester, A.
(1995) The core-periphery structure of the lexicon and constraints on reranking. Papers in Optimality Theory, 18, 181–209.Google Scholar
Koontz-Garboden, A.
(2004) Language contact and Spanish aspectual expression: a formal analysis. Lingua, 114(9–10), 1291–1330. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Koopman, H., & Sportiche, D.
(1991) The position of subjects. Lingua, 85(2), 211–258. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Kupisch, T.
(2007) Determiners in bilingual German-Italian children: What they tell us about the relation between language influence and language dominance. Bilingualism: Language and Cognition, 10(1), 57–78. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Landau, I., & Thornton, R.
(2011) Early child control. Lingua, 121(5), 920–941. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Legendre, G.
(2001) An introduction to Optimality Theory in syntax. In G. Legendre, J. Grimshaw, & S. Vikner (Eds.), Optimality-theoretic syntax (pp. 1–27). Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.Google Scholar
Legendre, G., Grimshaw, J. B., & Vikner, S.
(2001) Optimality-theoretic syntax. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.Google Scholar
Legendre, G., Hagstrom, P., Chen-Main, J., Tao, L., & Smolensky, P.
(2004) Deriving output probabilities in Child Mandarin from a Dual-Optimization grammar. Lingua, 114(6), 1147–1185. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Legendre, G., Hagstrom, P., Vainikka, A., & Todorova, M.
(2002) Partial constraint ordering in child French syntax. Language Acquisition, 10(3), 189–227. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Legendre, G., Putnam, M. T., Swart, H. de, & Zaroukian, E.
(Eds.) (2016) Optimality Theoretic Syntax, Semantics, and Pragmatics: From Uni- to Bidirectional Optimization. Oxford, United Kingdom: Oxford University Press. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Leopold, W.
(1949) Speech development of a bilingual child (Vol. 4). Evanston, IL: Northwestern University Press.Google Scholar
MacSwan, J.
(2000) The architecture of the bilingual language faculty: Evidence from intrasentential code switching. Bilingualism: Language and Cognition, 3(1), 37–54. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
(2016) Codeswitching and the timing of Lexical Insertion. Linguistic Approaches to Bilingualism, 6(6), 786–791. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Meisel, J. M.
(2001) The simultaneous acquisition of two first languages. Trends in Bilingual Acquisition 1, 11. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
(2007) On autonomous syntactic development in multiple first language acquisition. In Proceedings of the 31st Boston University conference on language development (Vol. 1, pp. 26–45).Google Scholar
Mishina-Mori, S.
(2005) Autonomous and interdependent development of two language systems in Japanese/English simultaneous bilinguals: Evidence from question formation. First Language, 25(3), 291–315. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Ornat, S. L.
(1999) La adquisición del lenguaje: nuevas perspectivas. In Psicolingüística del español (pp. 469–534). Trotta.Google Scholar
Ota, M.
(2003) The Development of Prosodic Structure in Early Words: Continuity, Divergence, and Change. John Benjamins Publishing. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Paradis, J., & Genesee, F.
(1996) Syntactic Acquisition in Bilingual Children: Autonomous or Interdependent? Studies in Second Language Acquisition, 18(1), 1–25. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Paradis, J., Crago, M., & Genesee, F.
(2003) Object clitics as a clinical marker of SLI in French: Evidence from French-English bilingual children. In Proceedings of the 27th Annual Boston University Conference on Language Development (Vol. 2, pp. 638–649).Google Scholar
Park, S. K.
(2008) The acquisition of wh-questions by Korean-English bilingual children: The role of crosslinguistic influence. Unpublished Master’s Thesis, Purdue University.Google Scholar
Patuto, M., Repetto, V., & Müller, N.
(2011) Delay and acceleration in bilingual first language acquisition: the same or different? In E. Rinke & T. Kupisch (Eds.), The development of grammar, language acquisition and diachronic change (pp. 231–259). Amsterdam: John Benjamins.Google Scholar
Pearson, B. Z.
(2007) Social factors in childhood bilingualism in the United States. Applied Psycholinguistics, 28(03), 399–410. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Pérez-Leroux, A., & Dalious, J.
(1998) The acquisition of Spanish interrogative inversion. Hispanic Linguistics, 10(1), 84–114.Google Scholar
Phillips, C.
(2010) Syntax at age two: Cross-linguistic differences. Language Acquisition, 17(1–2), 70–120. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Plunkett, B.
(1991) Inversion and early WH questions. Papers in the Acquisition of Wh 125, 153.Google Scholar
Poeppel, D., & Wexler, K.
(1993) The Full Competence Hypothesis of Clause Structure in Early German. Language, 69(1), 1. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Polinsky, M., & Kagan, O.
(2007) Heritage languages: In the ‘wild’ and in the classroom. Language and Linguistics Compass, 1(5), 368–395.Google Scholar
Pozzan, L.
(2011) Asking Questions in Learner English: First and Second Language Acquisition of Main and Embedded Interrogative Strucutures (Doctoral dissertation). City University of New York.Google Scholar
Prince, A., & Smolensky, P.
(2004) Optimality Theory: Constraint interaction in generative grammar. Hoboken, NJ: Wiley-Blackwell. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Radford, A.
(1996) Towards a structure-building model of acquisition. Generative Perspectives on Language Acquisition: Empirical Findings, Theoretical Considerations, and Crosslinguistic Comparisons, 43–89. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Rizzi, L.
(1996) Residual Verb Second and the Wh-Criterion. Parameters and Functional Heads: Essays in Comparative Syntax, 63–90.Google Scholar
Rowland, C. F., & Pine, J. M.
(2000) Subject-auxiliary inversion errors and wh-question acquisition:“What children do know?Journal of Child Language, 27(1), 157–181. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Rowland, C. F., Pine, J. M., Theakston, A. L., & Lieven, E. V.
(2005) The incidence of error in young children’s wh-questions. Journal of Speech, Language, and Hearing Research, 48(2), 384–404. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Sankoff, D., & Poplack, S.
(1981) A formal grammar for code-switching 1. Research on Language & Social Interaction, 14(1), 3–45.Google Scholar
Schindler, M., Legendre, G., & Mbaye, A.
(2008) Violations of the PF Interface Condition in Urban Wolof. In Proceedings from the Annual Meeting of the Chicago Linguistic Society (Vol. 44, pp. 169–184). Chicago Linguistic Society.Google Scholar
Schmitz, K., Patuto, M., & Müller, N.
(2012) The null-subject parameter at the interface between syntax and pragmatics: Evidence from bilingual German-Italian, German-French and Italian-French children. First Language, 32(1–2), 205–238. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Serrat, E., & Capdevila, M.
(2001) La adquisición de la interrogación: las interrogativas parciales en catalán y castellano. Infancia Y Aprendizaje, 24(1), 3–17. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Serratrice, L.
(2013) Cross-linguistic influence in bilingual development: Determinants and mechanisms. Linguistic Approaches to Bilingualism, 3(1), 3–25. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Smolensky, P.
(1996) The initial state and ‘richness of the base’ in Optimality Theory. Rutgers Optimality Archive 293.Google Scholar
Snyder, W.
(2011) Children’s grammatical conservatism: Implications for syntactic theory. In Proceedings of the 35th Annual Boston University Conference on Language Development (pp. 1–20).Google Scholar
Solà, J.
(1992) Agreement and subjects. Univ. Autònoma de Barcelona, Facultat de Filosofia i Lletres, Departament de Filologia Catalana.Google Scholar
Sorace, A., & Serratrice, L.
(2009) Internal and external interfaces in bilingual language development: Beyond structural overlap. International Journal of Bilingualism, 13(2), CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Sorace, A., Serratrice, L., Filiaci, F., & Baldo, M.
(2009) Discourse conditions on subject pronoun realization: Testing the linguistic intuitions of older bilingual children. Lingua, 119(3), 460–477. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Soriente, A.
(2007) Cross-Linguistic and Cognitive Structures in the Acquisition of wh-Questions in an Indonesian-Italian Bilingual Child. Cognitive Aspects of Bilingualism, 325–362. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Strik, N., & Pérez-Leroux, A. T.
(2011) Jij doe wat girafe?: Wh-movement and inversion in Dutch-French bilingual children. Linguistic Approaches to Bilingualism, 1(2), 175–205. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Stromswold, K. J.
(1990) Learnability and the acquisition of auxiliaries. Massachusetts Institute of Technology. Retrieved from http://​dspace​.mit​.edu​/handle​/1721​.1​/13715
Suñer, M.
(1994) V-movement and the licensing of argumental wh-phrases in Spanish. Natural Language & Linguistic Theory, 12(2), 335–372. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Tedick, D. J., Christian, D., & Fortune, T. W.
(2011) Immersion Education: Practices, Policies, Possibilities. Multilingual Matters.Google Scholar
Tesar, B., & Smolensky, P.
(1998) Learnability in Optimality Theory. Linguistic Inquiry, 29(2), 229–268. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Theakston, A. L., Lieven, E. V., Pine, J. M., & Rowland, C. F.
(2001) The role of performance limitations in the acquisition of verb-argument structure: An alternative account. Journal of Child Language, 28(1), 127–152. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Vainikka, A.
(1994) Case in the development of English syntax. Language Acquisition, 3(3), 257–325. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Vainikka, A., & Young-Scholten, M.
(1994) Direct access to X’-theory: evidence from Korean and Turkish adults learning German. Language Acquisition Studies in Generative Grammar 265316. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
(2013) Universal Minimal Structure: Evidence and theoretical ramifications. Linguistic Approaches to Bilingualism, 3(2), 180–211. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Vasilyeva, M., Waterfall, H., GáMez, P. B., GóMez, L. E., Bowers, E., & Shimpi, P.
(2010) Cross-linguistic syntactic priming in bilingual children. Journal of Child Language, 37(5), 1047–1064. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Villa-García, J., & Snyder, W.
(2009) On the acquisition of overt subjects, topics and wh-questions in Spanish. In Language Acquisition and Development: Proceedings of GALA (pp. 493–504).Google Scholar
Volterra, V., & Taeschner, T.
(1978) The acquisition and development of language by bilingual children. Journal of Child Language, 5(2), 311–326. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Wexler, K.
(1998) Very early parameter setting and the unique checking constraint: A new explanation of the optional infinitive stage. Lingua, 106(1), 23–79. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Williams, K. T.
(1997) Expressive Vocabulary Test – Second Edition (EVT-2). J. Am. Acad. Child Adolesc. Psychiatry 42, 864–872.Google Scholar
Yip, V., & Matthews, S.
(2000) Syntactic transfer in a Cantonese-English bilingual child. Bilingualism: Language and Cognition, 3(3), 193–208. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
(2007) The bilingual child: early development and language contact. Cambridge, UK ; New York: Cambridge University Press. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Cited by

Cited by 1 other publications

Putnam, Michael T.
2020. Separating vs. shrinking. Bilingualism: Language and Cognition 23:1  pp. 41 ff. Crossref logo

This list is based on CrossRef data as of 16 april 2021. Please note that it may not be complete. Sources presented here have been supplied by the respective publishers. Any errors therein should be reported to them.