References

References

Biberauer, T.
(2017) Factors 2 and 3: a principled approach. Cambridge Occasional Papers in Linguistics 101: 38–65.Google Scholar
Chomsky, N.
(1995) The Minimalist Program. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.Google Scholar
(2005) Three factors in language design. Linguistic Inquiry 361, 1–22. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Gervain, J. & Werker, J.
(2008) How infant speech perception contributes to language acquisition. Language and Linguistics Compass 2(6), 1149–1170. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Kidd, C., Piatandosi, S. & Aslin, R.
(2012) The Goldilocks Effect: human infants allocate attention to visual sequences that are neither too simple nor too complex. PLoS ONE 7(5), e36399. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Yang, C.
(2004) Universal Grammar, statistics, or both? Trends in Cognitive Science 101, 451–456. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
(2018) A formalist perspective on language acquisition. Linguistic Approaches to Bilingualism 6(6), 665–706. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Cited by

Cited by 2 other publications

Pérez-Leroux, Ana T.
2021. Children do not ignore (null objects): Against deficit accounts of the null object stage in language acquisition. Arborescences :10  pp. 127 ff. DOI logo
Yang, Charles
2018. Some consequences of the Tolerance Principle. Linguistic Approaches to Bilingualism 8:6  pp. 797 ff. DOI logo

This list is based on CrossRef data as of 23 april 2022. Please note that it may not be complete. Sources presented here have been supplied by the respective publishers. Any errors therein should be reported to them.