Article published In:
Linguistic Approaches to Bilingualism
Vol. 11:6 (2021) ► pp.817845
References
Aoun, J., & Li, A. Y.
(1989) Scope and constituency. Linguistic Inquiry, 20(2), 141–172.Google Scholar
Arnold, J. E., Wasow, T., Losongco, A., & Ginstrom, R.
(2000) Heaviness versus newness: The effects of structural complexity and discourse status on constituent ordering. Language, 761, 28–55. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Bel, A., & Rosado, E.
(2009) Person and number asymmetries in child Catalan and Spanish. In J. Grinstead (Ed.), Hispanic Child Languages (pp. 195–214). Amsterdam: John Benjamins Publishing. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Borer, H., & Wexler, K.
(1987) The maturation of syntax. In T. Roeper & K. Wexler (Eds.), Parameter Setting (pp. 123–172). Dordrecht: Reidel. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Bruhn de Garavito, J. L. S.
(2000) The syntax of Spanish multifunctional clitics and near-native competence (Doctoral dissertation). McGill University, Québec, Canada.Google Scholar
Campbell, A. L., & Tomasello, M.
(2001) The acquisition of English dative constructions. Applied Psycholinguistics, 22(2), 253–267. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Chomsky, N.
(1981) On the representation of form and function. The Linguistic Review, 11, 3–40. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Clark, E. V.
(2009) First language acquisition. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Cuervo, C.
(2003a) Datives at large (Doctoral dissertation). The Massachusetts Institute of Technology, Massachusetts, USA.Google Scholar
(2003b) Structural asymmetries but same word order: The dative alternation in Spanish. In A. M. Di Sciullo (Ed.), Asymmetry in Grammar. Volume I: Syntax and Semantics (pp. 117–144). Amsterdam: John Benjamins Publishing. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Czepluch, H.
(1982) Case theory and the dative construction. The Linguistic Review, 2(1), 1–38. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
De Houwer, A.
(1990) The acquisition of two languages from birth: A case study. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
(2005) Early bilingual acquisition: Focus on morphosyntax and the separate development hypothesis. In J. Kroll & A. De Groot (Eds.), The Handbook of Bilingualism (pp. 30–48). Oxford: Oxford University PressGoogle Scholar
Demonte, V.
(1994) Datives in Spanish. Working Papers in Linguistics, 4(1), 71–96.Google Scholar
(1995) Dative alternation in Spanish. Probus, 7(1), 5–30. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Dryer, M. S.
(1986) Primary objects, secondary objects and antidative. Language, 62(4), 808–845. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Genesee, F., Nicoladis, E., & Paradis, J.
(1995) Language differentiation in early bilingual development. Journal of Child Language, 22(3), 611–631. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Gropen, J., Pinker, S., Hollander, M., Goldberg, R., & Wilson, R.
(1989) The learnability and acquisition of the dative alternation in English. Language, 651, 203–257. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Gu, C. C.
(2010) Cross-linguistic influence in two directions: The acquisition of dative constructions in Cantonese-English bilingual children. International Journal of Bilingualism, 17(1), 87–103.Google Scholar
Haspelmath, M.
(2006) Ditransitive constructions in RRG and some other approaches. International Conference on Role and Reference Grammar, Leipzig, Germany.Google Scholar
Hulk, A., & Müller, N.
(2001) Bilingual first language acquisition at the interface between syntax and pragmatics. Bilingualism: Language and Cognition, 3(3), 227–244. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Larson, R. K.
(1988) On the double object construction. Linguistic Inquiry, 191, 335–391.Google Scholar
Legate, J. A., & Yang, C.
(2002) Empirical re-assessment of stimulus poverty arguments. The Linguistic Review, 191, 151–162. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
MacWhinney, B.
(2000) The CHILDES project: Tools for analyzing talk (third edition). Mahwah: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.Google Scholar
Marantz, A.
(1984) On the nature of grammatical relations. Cambridge: Massachusetts Institute of Technology Press.Google Scholar
(1993) Implications of asymmetries in double object constructions. In S. A. Mchombo (Ed.), Theoretical Aspects of Bantu Grammar (pp. 113–150). Stanford: Leland Stanford Junior University PressGoogle Scholar
Meisel, J. M.
(2004) The bilingual child. In T. K. Bathia & W. C. Ritchie (Eds.), The handbook of Bilingualism (pp. 91–113). Oxford: BlackwellGoogle Scholar
Mulder, R.
(1992) The aspectual nature of syntactic complementation. Leiden: Holland Institute of Generative Linguistics.Google Scholar
Oehrle, R. T.
(1976) The grammatical status of the dative alternation (Doctoral Dissertation), Massachusetts Institute of Technology, USA.Google Scholar
Paradis, J., Crago, E., & Genesee, F.
(2006) Domain-specific versus domain-general theories of the deficit in SLI: Object pronoun acquisition by French-English bilingual children. Language Acquisition, 13(1), 33–62. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Paradis, J., & Genesee, F.
(1996) Syntactic acquisition in bilingual children: Autonomous or interdependent? Studies in Second Language Acquisition, 181, 1–25. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Peccei, J. S.
(1999) Child language. London: Routledge.Google Scholar
Perlmutter, D. M.
(1990) Relational grammar. In E. A. Moravcsik & J. R. Wirth (Eds.), Syntax and Semantics: Current Approaches to Syntax 131 (pp. 195–229). Orlando Academic Press.Google Scholar
Pylkkänen, L.
(2002) Introducing arguments (Doctoral dissertation). The Massachusetts Institute of Technology, Massachusetts, USA.Google Scholar
Rowland, C.
(2014) Understanding child language acquisition. London: Routledge.Google Scholar
Ronjat, J.
(1913) Le développement du langage observé chez an enfant bilingue. Paris: Librairie Ancienne H. Champion.Google Scholar
Rothman, J., González Alonso, J., & Puig Mayenco, E.
(2019) Third language acquisition and linguistic transfer. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Sánchez Calderón, S., & Fernández Fuertes, R.
(2016) Dativizable or non-dativizable: That is the question? A syntactic-semantic analysis of English (non)-dativizable constructions in the production of a set of 2L1 English/Spanish simultaneous bilingual twins. Xjenza Online-Journal of the Malta Chamber of Scientists, 41, 44–57.Google Scholar
(2018) Which came first: The chicken or the egg? Ditransitive and passive constructions in the English production of simultaneous bilingual English children. ATLANTIS. A Journal of the Spanish Association for Anglo-American Studies, 40(1), 39–58. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Snyder, W.
(1995) A neo-davidsonian approach to resultatives, particles, and datives. In J. Beckman (Ed.), Proceedings of 25 North East Linguistic Society 251 (pp. 457–472). Amherst: Graduate Linguistic Student Association of the University of Massachusetts.Google Scholar
(2001) On the nature of syntactic variation: Evidence from complex predicates and complex word-formation. Language, 771, 324–342. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Snyder, K.
(2003) The relationship between form and function in ditransitive constructions (Doctoral dissertation), University of Pennsylvania, Philadelphia, USA.Google Scholar
Snyder, W., & Stromswold, K.
(1997) The structure and acquisition of English dative constructions. Linguistic Inquiry, 28(2), 281–317.Google Scholar
Torrens, V., & Wexler, K.
(2000) The acquisition of clitic doubling in Spanish. In S. M. Powers & C. Hamann (Eds.), The Acquisition of Scrambling and Cliticization. Studies in Theoretical Psycholinguistics 261 (pp. 279–297). Dordrecht: Springer. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Yang, C.
(2016) The price of linguistic productivity: How children learn to break the rules of language. Cambridge: Massachusetts Institute of Technology Press. DOI logoGoogle Scholar