Article in:
Linguistic Approaches to Bilingualism
Vol. 11:5 (2021) ► pp. 611639
References

References

Armon-Lotem, S., & Meir, N.
(2016) Diagnostic accuracy of repetition tasks for the identification of specific language impairment (SLI) in bilingual children: evidence from Russian and Hebrew. International Journal of Language and Communication Disorders, 51, 715–731. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Baguley, T.
(2012) Calculating and graphing within-subject confidence intervals for ANOVA. Behavior Research Methods, 44, 158–175. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Bartolotti, J., Marian, V., Schroeder, S. R., & Shook, A.
(2011) Bilingualism and inhibitory control influence statistical learning of novel word forms. Frontiers in Psychology, 2, 1–10. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Bates, D., Maechler, M., Bolker, B., & Walker, S.
(2015) Fitting linear mixed-effect models using lme4. Journal of Statistical Software, 67, 1–48. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Biedroń, A., & Szczepaniak, A.
(2012) Working memory and short-term memory abilities in accomplished multilinguals. The Modern Language Journal, 96, 290–306. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Blom, E., Küntay, A. C., Messer, M., Verhagen, J., & Leseman, P. P. M.
(2014) The benefits of being bilingual: Working memory in bilingual Turkish-Dutch children. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 128, 105–119. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Boerma, T., Chiat, S., Leseman, P. P. M., Timmermeister, M., Wijnen, F., & Blom, E.
(2015) A quasi-universal nonword repetition task as a diagnostic tool for bilingual children learning Dutch as a second language. Journal of Speech, Language, and Hearing Research, 58, 1747–1760. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Bonifacci, P., Giombini, L., Bellocchi, S., & Contento, S.
(2011) Speed of processing, anticipation, inhibition and working memory in bilinguals. Developmental Science, 14, 256–269. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
de Bree, E. H., Verhagen, J., Kerkhoff, A. O., Doedens, W. J., & Unsworth, S.
(2017) Language learning from inconsistent input: Bilingual and monolingual toddlers compared. Infant and Child Development, 26, e1996. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Bulgarelli, F., Lebkuecher, A. L. & Weiss, D. J.
(2018) Statistical learning in bilingualism. Language, Speech, and Hearing Services in Schools, 49, 740–753. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Chambers, J. M., Freeny, A. R., & Heiberger, R. M.
(1992) Analysis of variance; designed experiments. In Chambers, J. M., & Hastie, T. J. (Eds.), Statistical Models in S. Wadsworth & Brooks/Cole.Google Scholar
le Clercq, C. M. P., van der Schroeff, M. P., Rispens, J. E., Ruytjens, L., Goedegebure, A., van Ingen, G., & Franken, M-C.
(2017) Shortened nonword repetition task (NWR-S): A simple, quick and less expensive outcome to identify children with combined specific language and reading impairment. Journal of Speech, Language, and Hearing Research, 60, 2241–2248. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Delcenserie, A., & Genesee, F.
(2017) The effects of age of acquisition on verbal memory in bilinguals. International Journal of Bilingualism, 21, 600–616. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Duncan, T. S., & Paradis, J.
(2016) English language learners’ nonword repetition performance: The influence of age, L1 vocabulary size, length of L2 exposure, and L1 phonology. Journal of Speech, Language, and Hearing Research, 59, 39–48. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Dutilh, G., Vandekerckhove, J., Forstmann, B. U., Keuleerst, E., Brysbaert, M., & Wagenmakers, E-J.
(2012) Testing theories of post-error slowing. Attention, Perception and Psychophysics, 74, 454–465. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Erickson, L. C., & Thiessen, E. D.
(2015) Statistical learning of language: Theory, validity, and predictions of a statistical learning account of language acquisition. Developmental Review, 37, 66–108. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Franco, A., Eberlen, J., Destrebecqz, A., Cleeremans, A., & Bertels, J.
(2015) Rapid serial auditory presentation: A new measure of statistical learning in speech segmentation. Experimental Psychology, 62, 346–351. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Fernandes, M. A., Craik, F. I. M., Bialystok, E., & Kreuger, S.
(2007) Effects of bilingualism, aging, and semantic relatedness on memory under divided attention. Canadian Journal of Experimental Psychology, 61, 128–141. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Gathercole, S. E.
(2006) Keynote article: Nonword repetition and word learning: The nature of the relationship. Applied Psycholinguistics, 27, 513–543. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Grama, I. C., Kerkhoff, A. O., & Wijnen, F. N. K.
(2016) Gleaning structure from sound: The role of prosodic contrast in learning non-adjacent dependencies. Journal of Psycholinguistic Research, 45, 1427–1449. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Hautus, M. J.
(1995) Corrections for extreme proportions and their biasing effects on estimated values of d’ . Behavior Research Methods, 27, 46–51.Google Scholar
Hirosh, Z., & Degani, T.
(2017) Direct and indirect effects of multilingualism on novel language learning: An integrative review. Psychonomic Bulletin Review, 25, 892–916. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Hunt, R. H., & Aslin, R. N.
(2001) Statistical learning in a serial reaction time task: Access to separable statistical cues by individual learners. Journal of Experimental Psychology, 130, 658–680. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Jaeger, T. F.
(2010) Post to HLP/Jaeger lab blog. https://​hlplab​.wordpress​.com​/2009​/05​/14​/random​-effect​-structure/, accessed September 2019.
Kapa, L. L., & Colombo, J.
(2014) Executive function predicts artificial language learning. Journal of Memory and Language, 76, 237–252. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Kaushanskaya, M.
(2012) Cognitive mechanisms of word learning in bilingual and monolingual adults: The role of verbal short-term memory. Bilingualism: Language & Cognition, 15, 470–489. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Keuleers, E., Brysbaert, M., & New, B.
(2010) SUBTLEX-NL: A new frequency measure for Dutch words based on film subtitles. Behavior Research Methods, 42, 643–650. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Kovács, A. & Mehler, J.
(2009) Flexible learning of multiple speech structures in bilingual infants. Science, 325, 611–612. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Kuo, L. J., & Anderson, R. C.
(2010) Beyond cross-language transfer: Reconceptualizing the impact of early bilingualism on phonological awareness. Scientific Studies of Reading, 14, 365–385. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
(2012) Effects of early bilingualism on learning phonological regularities in a new language. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 111, 455–467. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Kuznetsova, A., Brockhoff, P. B., & Christensen, R. H. B.
(2017) lmerTest Package: Tests in Linear Mixed Effects Models. Journal of Statistical Software, 82, 1–26. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Lammertink, I., van Witteloostuijn, M., Boersma, P., Wijnen, F., & Rispens, J.
(2019) Auditory statistical learning in children: Novel insights from an online measure. Applied Psycholinguistics, 40, 279–302. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Lum, J. A. G., Conti-Ramsden, G., Morgan, A. T., & Ullman, M. T.
(2014) Procedure learning deficits in specific language impairment (SLI): a meta-analysis of serial reaction time task performance. Cortex, 51, 1–10. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Macmillan, N. A., & Creelman, C. D.
(2005) Detection theory: A user’s guide (2nd edition). Mahwah, New Jersey: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.Google Scholar
McKean, C., Letts, C., & Howard, D.
(2013) Developmental change is key to understanding primary language impairment: The case of phonotactic probability and nonword repetition. Journal of Speech, Language, and Hearing Research, 56, 1579–1594. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Messer, M. H., Leseman, P. P. M., Boom, J., & Mayo, A. Y.
(2010) Phonotactic probability effect in nonword recall and its relationship with vocabulary in monolingual and bilingual preschoolers. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 105, 306–323. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Misyak, J. B., & Christiansen, M. H.
(2012) Statistical language and language: An individual differences study. Language Learning, 62, 302–331. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Nissen, M. J., & Bullemer, P.
(1987) Attentional requirements of learning: evidence from performance measures. Cognitive Psychology, 19, 1e32. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Onnis, L., Chun, W. E., & Lou-Magnuson, M.
(2017) Improved statistical learning abilities in adult bilinguals. Bilingualism: Language and Cognition, 21, 427–433. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Parra, M., Hoff, E., & Core, C.
(2011) Relations among language exposure, verbal short-term memory, and language development in Spanish-English bilingually developing two-year-olds. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 108, 113–125. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Park, J. S., Miller, C., Rosenbaum, D. A., Sanjeevan, T., van Hell, J. G., Weiss, D. J., & Mainela-Arnold, E.
(2018) Bilingualism and procedural learning in typically developing children and children with language impairment. Journal of Speech, Language, and Hearing Research, 61, 634–644. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Perruchet, P. & Tillmann, B.
(2010) Exploiting multiple sources of information in learning an artificial language: Human data and modeling. Cognitive Science, 34, 255–285. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Poepsel, T. J., & Weiss, D.
(2016) The influence of bilingualism on statistical word learning. Cognition, 152, 9–19. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Powell, M. J. D.
(2009) The BOBYQA algorithm for bound constrained optimization without derivatives (Report). Department of Applied Mathematics and Theoretical Physics, Cambridge University. http://​www​.damtp​.cam​.ac​.uk​/user​/na​/NA​_papers​/NA2009​_06​.pdf, accessed September 2019.
R Core Team
(2015) R: A language environment for statistical computing. R Foundation for Statistical Computing, Vienna, Austria.Google Scholar
Rispens, J., & Baker, A.
(2012) Nonword repetition: the relative contributions of phonological short-term memory and phonological representations in children with language and hearing impairment. Journal of Speech, Language, and Hearing Research, 55, 683–694. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Stanislaw, H., & Todorov, N.
(1999) Calculation of signal detection measures. Behavior Research Methods, Instruments and Computers: A Journal of the Psychonomic Society, 31, 137–149. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Thiessen, E. D.
(2017) What’s statistical about learning? Insights from modelling statistical learning as a set of memory processes. Philosophical transactions of the Royal Society B, 372, 1711. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Vicari, S., Marotti, L., Menghini, D., Molinari, M., & Petrosini, L.
(2003) Implicit learning deficit in children with developmental dyslexia. Neuropsychologica, 41, 108–114. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Wang, T., & Saffran, J. R.
(2014) Statistical learning of a tonal language: the influence of bilingualism and previous linguistic experience. Frontiers in Psychology, 5, 953. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Weiss, D., Gerfen, C., & Mitchel, A.
(2009) Speech segmentation in a simulated bilingual environment: A challenge for statistical learning? Language Learning and Development, 5, 30–49. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Yim, D., & Rudoy, J.
(2013) Implicit statistical learning and language skills in bilingual children. Journal of Speech, Language, and Hearing Research, 56, 310–323. CrossrefGoogle Scholar