Previous studies have shown that bilingual children typically score more poorly on nonword repetition (NWR) tasks than monolingual peers, which has been attributed to bilinguals’ lower proficiency in the language that the NWR task is based on. To enable fairer assessments of bilingual children, Cross-Linguistic NWR tasks (CL-NWR tasks) have been developed that are based on the linguistic properties of many languages. The aim of this study is to investigate whether young children’s performance on a CL-NWR is less dependent on existing knowledge of a specific language than performance on a Language-Specific (Dutch-based) NWR (LS-NWR). Bilingual and multilingual two- and three-year-olds (N = 216) completed a CL-NWR and LS-NWR, as well as a Dutch receptive vocabulary task. Parents reported the number of languages children spoke other than Dutch. Results of linear mixed-effect regressions showed that Dutch vocabulary scores related to performance on the CL-NWR task less strongly than to performance on the LS-NWR task. The number of non-Dutch languages spoken did not differentially relate to performance on the two tasks. These findings indicate that CL-NWR tasks – at least as used here – allow for more language-neutral NWR assessments within linguistically diverse samples, already at toddler age.
Adams, A. M., & Gathercole, S. E. (1996). Phonological memory and spoken language development in young children. Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 491, 216–233.
Archibald, L. (2008). The promise of nonword repetition as a clinical tool. Canadian Journal of Speech-Language Pathology and Audiology, 321, 21–28.
Antonijevic-Elliott, S., Lyons, R., Pat O’ Malley, M., Meir, N., Haman, E., Banasik, N., Carroll, C., McMenamin, R., Rodden, M., & Fitzmaurice, Y. (2020). Language assessment of monolingual and multilingual children using non-word and sentence repetition tasks, Clinical Linguistics & Phonetics, 341, 293–311.
Barr, D. J., Levy, R., Scheepers, C. & Tily, H. J. (2013). Random effects structure for confirmatory hypothesis testing: Keep it maximal. Journal of Memory and Language, 681, 255–278.
Bates, D., Mächler, M., Bolker, B., & Walker, S. (2015). Fitting linear mixed-effect models using lme4. Journal of Statistical Software, 671,
Beers, M. (1995). The phonology of normally developing and language-impaired children. Doctoral dissertation, University of Amsterdam.
Boerma, T., Chiat, S., Leseman, P. P. M., Timmermeister, M., Wijnen, F. N. K., & Blom, W. B. T. (2015). A cross-linguistic nonword repetition task as a diagnostic tool for bilingual children learning Dutch as a second language. Journal of Speech, Language and Hearing Research, 581, 1747–1760.
Brandeker, M., & Thordardottir, E. (2015). Language exposure in bilingual toddlers: Performance on nonword repetition and lexical tasks. American Journal of Speech- Language Pathology, 241, 126–138.
Briscoe, J., Bishop, D. V. M., & Norbury, C. F. (2001). Phonological processing, language, and literacy: A comparison of children with mild-to-moderate sensorineural hearing loss and those with specific language impairment. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 421, 329–340.
Chiat, S. (2015). Nonword repetition. In S. Armon-Lotem, J. de Jong, & N. Meir (Eds.), Methods for assessing multilingual children: Disentangling bilingualism from language impairment (pp. 125–150). Multilingual Matters.
Chiat, S., & Polišenská, K. (2016). A framework for crosslinguistic nonword repetition tests: Effects of bilingualism and socioeconomic status on children’s performance. Journal of Speech, Language and Hearing Research, 591, 1179–1189.
Chiat, S., & Roy, P. (2007). The Preschool Repetition Test: An evaluation of performance in typically developing and clinically referred children. Journal of Speech, Language, and Hearing Research, 501, 429–443.
Coady, J. A., & Aslin, R. N. (2004). Young children’s sensitivity to probabilistic phonotactics in the developing lexicon. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 891, 183–213.
Coady, J. A., & Evans, J. L. (2008). Uses and interpretations of non-word repetition tasks in children with and without specific language impairment (SLI). International Journal of Communication Disorders, 431, 1–40.
Conti-Ramsden, G., & Hesketh, A. (2003). Risk markers for SLI: A study of young language- learning children. International Journal of Language and Communication Disorders, 381, 251–263.
Crawley, M. J. (2012). The R book. John Wiley & Sons.
Daelemans, W., Gillis, S., & Durieux, G. (1994). The acquisition of stress: A data-oriented approach. Computational Linguistics, 201, 421–451.
Dispaldro, M., Leonard, L. B., Deevy, P. (2013). Real word and nonword repetition in Italian- Speaking children with specific language impairment: A study of diagnostic accuracy, Journal of Speech, Language, and Hearing Research, 561, 323–336.
Duncan, T. S., & Paradis, J. (2013). English language learners’ nonword repetition performance: The influence of age, L2, vocabulary size, length of L2 exposure, and L1 phonology. Journal of Speech Language and Hearing Research, 591, 39–48.
Dunn, L., & Dunn, L. M. (2005). Peabody Picture Vocabulary Test-III-NL. Nederlandse versie door Liesbeth Schlichting [Dutch version by Liesbeth Schlichting]. Harcourt Assessment B.V.
Ebert, K. D., Pham, G., Kohnert, K. (2014). Lexical profiles of bilingual children with primary language impairment. Bilingualism, 171, 766–783.
Engel de Abreu, P. M. J. (2011). Working memory in multilingual children: Is there a bilingual effect?Memory, 191, 529–537.
Engel de Abreu, P. M., Baldassi, M., Puglisi, M. L., & Befi-Lopes, D. M. (2013). Cross-linguistic and cross-cultural effects on verbal working memory and vocabulary: Testing language- minority children with an immigrant background. Journal of Speech, Language, and Hearing Research, 561, 630–642.
Fikkert, P. (1994). On the acquisition of prosodic structure. Doctoral dissertation, University of Leiden.
Gathercole, S. E. (1995). Is nonword repetition a test of phonological memory or long-term knowledge? It all depends on the nonwords. Memory and Cognition, 231, 83–94.
Gathercole, S. E. (2006). Nonword repetition and word learning: The nature of the relationship. Applied Psycholinguistics, 271, 513–543.
Gathercole, S. E., & Baddeley, A. D. (1989). Evaluation of the role of phonological STM in the development of vocabulary in children: A longitudinal study. Journal of Memory and Language, 281, 200–213.
Gathercole, S., & Baddeley, A. (1990). Phonological memory deficits in language disordered s children: Is there a causal connection?Journal of Memory and Language, 291, 336–360.
Gibson, T., Summers, C., Peña, E. D., Bedore, L. M., Gillam, R. B., & Bohman, T. (2015). The role of language structure and experience in bilingual children’s nonword repetition performance. Bilingualism: Language and Cognition. 181, 551–560.
Graf-Estes, K., Evans, J. L., Else-Quest, N. M. (2007). Differences in the nonword repetition performance of children with and without specific language impairment: a meta-analysis. Journal of Speech Language and Hearing Research, 501, 177–195.
Gray, S. (2003). Diagnostic accuracy and test-retest reliability of nonword repetition and digit span tasks administered to preschool children with specific language impairment. Journal of Communication Disorders, 361, 129–151.
Grimm, A., & Schulz, P. (2014). Specific language impairment and early second language acquisition: The risk of over- and underdiagnosis. Child Indicators Research, 71, 821–84.
Gutiérrez-Clellen, V. F., & Simon-Cerejido, G. (2010). Using nonword repetition tasks for the identification of language impairment in Spanish-English-speaking children: Does the language of assessment matter?Learning Disabilities Research & Practice, 251, 48–58.
Haspelmath, M. (2006). Against markedness (and what to replace it with). Journal of Linguistics, 421, 25–70.
Hoff, E., Core, C., & Bridges, K. (2008). Nonword repetition assesses phonological memory and is related to vocabulary development in one-year-olds. Journal of Child Language, 351, 903–916.
Hoff, E., Core, C., Place, S., Rumiche, R., Senor, M., & Parra, M. (2012). Dual language exposure and early bilingual development. Journal of Child Language, 391, 1–27.
Jones, G. (2016). The influence of children’s language exposure to language from two to six years: The case of nonword repetition. Cognition, 1531, 79–88.
Kaushanskaya, M. (2012). Cognitive mechanisms of word learning in bilingual and monolingual adults: The role of phonological memory. Bilingualism: Language and Cognition, 151, 470–489.
Keydeniers, D., Aalberse, S., Andringa, S., & Kuiken, F. (2021). Bilingual daycares in the Netherlands: an analysis of the implementation of bilingual input and underlying ideologies. Current Issues in Language Planning.
Kohnert, K., Windsor, J., & Yim, D. (2006). Do language-based processing tasks separate children with language impairment from typical bilinguals?Learning Disabilities Research & Practice, 211, 19–29.
Masoura, E. V. & Gathercole, S. E. (1999). Phonological short-term memory and foreign language learning. International Journal of Psychology, 341, 383–388.
Meir, N., & Armon-Lotem, S. (2017). Independent and combined effects of socioeconomic status (SES) and bilingualism on children’s vocabulary and verbal short-term memory. Frontiers in Psychology, 81: 1442.
Messer, M. H., Leseman, P. P. M., Boom, J., & Mayo, A. Y. (2010). Phonotactic probability effect in nonword recall and its relation with vocabulary in monolingual and bilingual preschoolers. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 1051, 306–323.
Metsala, J. L., & Chisholm, G. M. (2010). The influence of lexical status and neighborhood density on children’s nonword repetition. Applied Psycholinguistics, 311, 489–506.
Munson, B., Kurtz, B. A., & Windsor, J. (2005). The influence of vocabulary size, phonotactic probability, and wordlikeness on nonword repetitions of children with and without specific language impairment. Journal of Speech, Language, and Hearing Research, 481, 1033–1047.
Parra, M., Hoff, E., & Core, C. (2011). Relations among language exposure, phonological memory, and language development in Spanish-English bilingually developing 2-year- olds. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 1081, 113–125.
Powell, M. J. D. (2009). The BOBYQA algorithm for bound constrained optimization without derivatives. Seminar scholar. [URL]
R Core Team. (2019). R: A language and environment for statistical computing. R Foundation for Statistical Computing. [URL]
Rispens, J., & Baker, A. (2012). Nonword repetition: the relative contributions of phonological short-term memory and phonological representations in children with language and hearing impairment. Journal of Speech, Language, and Hearing Research, 551, 683–694.
Roy, P., & Chiat, S. (2004). A prosodically controlled word and nonword repetition task for 2-to-4-year-olds: Evidence from typically developing children. Journal of Speech, Language, and Hearing Research, 471, 223–234.
Schad, D. J., Vasishth, S., Hohenstein, S., & Kliegl, R. (2020). How to capitalize on a priori contrasts in linear (mixed) models: A tutorial. Journal of Memory and Language, 1101, 104038.
Thorn, A., & Gathercole, S. E. (1999). Language-specific knowledge and short-term memory in bilingual and non-bilingual children. The Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology Section A, 521, 303–324.
Thorn, A., Gathercole, S. E., & Frankish, C. R. (2005). Redintegration and the benefits of long- term knowledge in verbal short-term memory: An evaluation of Schweickert’s (1993) multinomial processing tree model. Cognitive Psychology, 501, 133–158.
Toombs, M., Singh, S., & Hayden, M. (1981). Markedness of features in the articulatory substitutions of children. Journal of Speech and Hearing Disorders, 461, 184–191.
Unsworth, S. (2015). Amount of exposure as a proxy for dominance in bilingual language acquisition. In C. Silva-Corvalan & J. Treffers-Daller (Eds.), Language dominance in bilinguals: Issues of measurement and operationalization (pp. 156–173). Cambridge University Press.
Verhagen, J. & Leseman, P. P. M. (2016). How do verbal short-term memory and working memory relate to the acquisition of vocabulary and grammar? A comparison between first and second language learners. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 1411, 65–82.
Verhagen, J., & Boom, J., Mulder, H., de Bree, E. H., & Leseman, P. P. M. (2019). Reciprocal relationships between nonword repetition and vocabulary during the preschool years. Developmental Psychology, 551, 1125–1137.
White, M. J. (2019). The development of English proficiency and working memory in 5–6 year old ELLs in their first year of formal education, International Journal of Bilingual Education and Bilingualism.
Windsor, J., Kohnert, K., Lobitz, K. F., & Pham, G. T. (2010). Cross-language nonword repetition by bilingual and monolingual children. American Journal of Speech-Language Pathology, 191, 298–310.
Zipf, G. K. (1935). (reprinted 1965). The psycho-biology of language. MIT Press.
2024. L1–L2 phonological skills predict L2 early literacy in bilinguals with developmental language disorder. Learning and Individual Differences 112 ► pp. 102458 ff.
2024. Individual variation in bilingual vocabulary in preschoolers with developmental language disorder. Research in Developmental Disabilities 147 ► pp. 104695 ff.
Sczepurek, Nina-Sophie, Suzanne P. Aalberse & Josje Verhagen
2022. Multilingual Children’s Motivations to Code-Switch: A Qualitative Analysis of Code-Switching in Dutch-English Bilingual Daycares. Languages 7:4 ► pp. 274 ff.
Verhagen, Josje, Folkert Kuiken & Sible Andringa
2022. Family language patterns in bilingual families and relationships with children’s language outcomes. Applied Psycholinguistics 43:5 ► pp. 1109 ff.
This list is based on CrossRef data as of 16 september 2024. Please note that it may not be complete. Sources presented here have been supplied by the respective publishers.
Any errors therein should be reported to them.