Article published In:
Linguistic Approaches to Bilingualism: Online-First ArticlesThe acquisition of consonant clusters and word stress by early second language learners of German
Evidence for cross-linguistic influence?
This study compared word-prosodic abilities of early second language learners (eL2) and monolingual learners of
German. We examined the production of word-initial and word-final clusters and the placement of stress and analyzed potential
effects of cross-linguistic influence (CLI). Monolingual German-speaking children (n = 38) and eL2-learners of
German (n = 26; age of onset to German 24 to 41 months) aged between 53 and 60 months completed a pseudoword
repetition task following the metrical and phonotactic constraints of German. We collected background information via parental
questionnaires. The eL2-learners acquired 12 different L1s. To explore the effects of CLI, we grouped the heritage languages by
the number of consonants permitted in word-initial and word-final position, the segmental make-up of clusters, and stress
patterns. The production accuracy of word-initial clusters and word stress was very high, indicating a high degree of maturation
and showing no effects of CLI. In contrast, the production accuracy of word-final clusters was lower and effects of CLI were
found, presumably related to smaller sonority distances compared to word-initial clusters. The study contributes empirically to
the under-investigated area of eL2 word-prosodic development.
Keywords: child second language learners, production of word stress, cross-linguistic interaction, German, production of consonant clusters
Article outline
- 1.Introduction
- 2.Phonological properties of clusters and word stress
- 2.1Consonant clusters
- 2.2Word stress
- 3.The development of consonant clusters and word stress in monolingual and bilingual children
- 3.1Cross-linguistic influences in bilingual phonological acquisition
- 3.2Acquisition of consonant clusters
- 3.3Acquisition of word stress
- 3.4Summary of previous findings and open questions
- 4.Predictions
- 4.1Crosslinguistic interaction: Predictions for word-initial consonant clusters
- 4.2Crosslinguistic interaction: Predictions for word-final consonant clusters
- 4.3Crosslinguistic interaction: Predictions for stress shifts
- 4.4Predictions regarding the connection of CLI and maturation
- 5.Method
- 5.1Participants
- 5.2Material
- 5.3Procedure
- 5.4Data transcription
- 5.5Data coding
- 5.6Statistical analysis
- 6.Results
- 6.1Monolingual vs. eL2-learners
- 6.2Cross-linguistic interaction: Word-initial clusters
- 6.3Cross-linguistic interaction: Word-final clusters
- 6.4Cross-linguistic interaction: Stress shifts
- 7.Discussion
- 7.1Consonant clusters
- 7.2Stress shifts
- 8.Conclusions
- Data availability statement
- Conflict of interest statement
- Acknowledgements
- Notes
-
References
Published online: 11 December 2023
https://doi.org/10.1075/lab.21026.gri
https://doi.org/10.1075/lab.21026.gri
References (62)
Altmann, H. (2006). The
perception and production of second language stress: A cross-linguistic study [PhD
Dissertation], Stuttgart. [URL]
Arias, J., & Lleó, C. (2009). Comparing
the representation of iambs by monolingual German, monolingual Spanish and bilingual German-Spanish
children. In K. Braunmüller & J. House (Eds.), Convergence
and divergence in language contact
situations (pp. 205–234). Benjamins.
Baker, W., Trofimovich, P., Flege, J. E., Mack, M., & Halter, R. (2008). Child-adult
differences in second-language phonological learning: The role of cross-language
similarity. Language and
Speech,
51
(4), 317–342.
Barlow, J. A. (2016). Sonority
in acquisition: A review. In M. J. Ball & N. Müller (Eds.), Studies
in Phonetics and Phonology. Challenging sonority: Cross-linguistic
evidence (pp. 295–336). Equinox.
Bunta, F., & Ingram, D. (2007). The
acquisition of speech rhythm by bilingual spanish- and english-speaking 4- and 5-year-old
children. Journal of Speech, Language, and Hearing
Research,
50
(4), 999–1014.
Clausen, M. C., & Fox-Boyer, A. (2017). Phonological
development of Danish-speaking children: A normative cross-sectional study. Clinical
Linguistics &
Phonetics,
31
(6), 440–458.
De Almeida, L., Ferré, S., Morin, E., Prévost, P., dos Santos, C., Tuller, L. et al. (2017). Language
Impairment in Bilingual Children. Linguistic Approaches to
Bilingualism
7
(3–4), 331–358.
de Almeida, L., Ferré, S., Barthez, M. A., & dos Santos, C. (2019). What
do monolingual and bilingual children with and without SLI produce when phonology is too
complex? First
Language,
39
(2), 158–176.
Demuth, K., & Fee, J. (1995). Minimal
prosodic words in early phonological development. Ms, Brown University and Dalhousie University.
Demuth, K., & Kehoe, M. M. (2006). The
acquisition of word-final clusters in French. Catalan Journal of
Linguistics,
5
1, 59–81.
Domahs, U., Genc, S., Knaus, J., Wiese, R., & Kabak, B. (2013). Processing
(un-)predictable word stress: ERP evidence from Turkish. Language and Cognitive
Processes,
28
(3), 335–354.
Dupoux, E., Peperkamp, S., & Sebastián-Gallés, N. (2001). A
robust method to study stress “deafness”. The Journal of the Acoustical Society of
America,
110
1, 1606–1618.
Elsen, H. (1991). Erstspracherwerb:
Der Erwerb des deutschen Lautsystems. Deutscher Universitätsverlag.
Fabiano-Smith, L., & Goldstein, B. A. (2010). Phonological
acquisition in bilingual Spanish–English speaking children. Journal of Speech, Language, and
Hearing
Research,
53
1, 160–178.
Féry, C. (2004). Phonologie
des Deutschen: Eine optimalitätstheoretische Einführung. Linguistics in Potsdam: Vol.
7. Universitäts-Verlag.
Fikkert, P. (1994). On
the acquisition of prosodic structure. HIL
dissertations: Vol. 61. ICG Printing.
Flege, J. E. (1995). Second-language
speech learning: Theory, findings, and problems. In: Strange, W. (ed)., Speech
perception and linguistic experience. Issues in cross-language
research (pp. 233–277). York Press.
Fox, A. V., & Dodd, B. (1999). Der
Erwerb des phonologischen Systems in der deutschen Sprache. Sprache Stimme
Gehör,
23
1, 183–191.
Fox-Boyer, A. (2016). Kindliche
Aussprachestörungen: Phonologischer Erwerb, Differenzialdiagnostik,
Therapie. Schulz-Kirchner.
Fox-Boyer, A., Fricke, S., & Albrecht, K. M. (2020). Phonologische
Prozesse in der deutschen Sprache türkisch-deutsch bilingualer Kinder. Sprache Stimme
Gehör,
44
(04), 216–223.
Giegerich, H. J. (2009). Metrical
phonology and phonological structure: German and English. Cambridge University Press.
Gildersleeve-Neumann, C. E., Kester, E. S., Davis, B. L., & Peña, E. D. (2008). English
speech sound development in preschool-aged children from bilingual English–Spanish
environments. Language, Speech, and Hearing Services in
Schools,
39
(3), 314–328.
Goldstein, B. A., Fabiano, L., & Washington, P. S. (2005). Phonological
skills in predominantly English-speaking, predominantly Spanish-speaking, and Spanish-English bilingual
children. Language, Speech, and Hearing Services in
Schools,
36
(3), 201–218.
Goldstein, B. A., & Washington, P. S. (2001). An
initial investigation of phonological patterns in typically developing 4-year-old Spanish-English bilingual
children. Language, Speech, and Hearing Services in
Schools,
32
1, 153–164.
Grimm, A. (2010). The
development of early prosodic word structure in child German: Simplex words and
compounds. Ph.D. dissertation, University of Potsdam. [URL]
Grimm, A. & Schulz, P. (2014). Specific
language impairment and early second language acquisition: The risk of over- and
underdiagnosis. Child Indicator
Research
7
(4), 821–841.
Holm, A., & Dodd, B. (1999). A
longitudinal study of the phonological development of two Cantonese–English bilingual
children. Applied
Psycholinguistics,
20
(3), 349–376.
Holm, A., Dodd, B., Stow, C., & Pert, S. (1999). Identification
and differential diagnosis of phonological disorder in bilingual children. Language
Testing,
16
(3), 271–292.
Keffala, B., Barlow, J. A., & Rose, S. (2018). Interaction
in Spanish–English bilinguals’ acquisition of syllable structure. International Journal of
Bilingualism,
22
(1), 16–37.
Kehoe, M. M. (2015). Cross-linguistic
interaction: A retrospective and prospective view. In E. Barbatsouli & D. Ingram (Eds.), Proceedings
of the international symposium of monolingual and bilingual speech
2015 (pp. 141–167). [URL]
Kehoe, M. M., & Girardier, C. (2020). What
factors influence phonological production in French-speaking bilingual children, aged three to six
years? Journal of Child
Language,
47
(5), 945–981.
Kehoe, M. M., & Havy, M. (2019). Bilingual
phonological acquisition: The influence of language-internal, language-external, and lexical
factors. Journal of Child
Language,
46
1, 1–42.
Kehoe, M. M., & Stoel-Gammon, C. (1997). The
acquisition of prosodic structure: An investigation of current accounts of children’s prosodic
development. Language,
73
(1), 113–144.
Keshavarz, M. H., & Ingram, D. (2002). The
early phonological development of a bilingual Farsi-English child. International Journal of
Bilingualism,
6
(3), 255–269.
Kirk, C., & Demuth, K. (2005). Asymmetries
in the acquisition of word-initial and word-final consonant clusters. Journal of Child
Language,
32
(4), 709–734.
Lausecker, A., Grimm, A., & Schulz, P. (2021). Der
Erwerb der Wortbetonungsregeln im frühen Zweitspracherwerb des
Deutschen. In A.-L. Scherger, B. Lütke, Montanari, Elke, A. Müller, & J. Richard-Brede (Eds.), Deutsch
als Zweitsprache – Forschungsfelder und
Ergebnisse. (pp. 163–183). Fillibach bei Klett.
Lleó, C. (2006). The
acquisition of prosodic word structures in Spanish by monolingual and Spanish-German bilingual
children. Language and
Speech,
49
(2), 205–229.
Lleó, C., & Prinz, M. (1996). Consonant
clusters in child phonology and the directionality of syllable structure assignment. Journal of
Child
Language,
23
(1), 31–56.
Marshall, C. R., Harris, J., & van der Lely, H. K. J. (2003). The
nature of phonological representations in children with Grammatical-Specific Language
Impairment. In D. Hall, T. Markopoulos, A. Salamoura, & S. Skoufaki (Eds.), The
University of Cambridge First Postgraduate Conference in Language
Research (pp. 511–517). Cambridge Institute of Language Research.
Mayr, R., Howells, G., & Lewis, R. (2015). Asymmetries
in phonological development: The case of word-final cluster acquisition in Welsh-English bilingual
children. Journal of Child
Language,
42
(1), 146–179.
Mayr, R., Jones, D., & Mennen, I. (2014). 1.
Speech learning in bilinguals: Consonant cluster
acquisition. In E. M. Thomas & I. Mennen (Eds.), Advances
in the Study of
Bilingualism (pp. 3–24). Channel View Publications.
McLeod, S., van Doorn, J., & Reed, V. A. (2001). Normal
acquisition of consonant clusters. American Journal of Speech-Language
Pathology,
10
(2), 99–110.
Mok, P. P. (2011). The
acquisition of speech rhythm by three-year-old bilingual and monolingual children: Cantonese and
English. Bilingualism: Language and
Cognition,
14
(4), 458–472.
Mołczanow, J., Iskra, E., Dragoy, O., Wiese, R., & Domahs, U. (2019). Default
stress assignment in Russian: evidence from acquired surface
dyslexia. Phonology,
36
(1), 61–90.
Morrow, A., Goldstein, B. A., Gilhool, A., & Paradis, J. (2014). Phonological
skills in English language learners. Language, Speech, and Hearing Services in
Schools,
45
(1), 26–39.
Paradis, J. (2001). Do
bilingual two-year-olds have separate phonological systems? International Journal of
Bilingualism,
5
(1), 19–38.
Paradis, J., & Genesee, F. (1996). Syntactic
acquisition in bilingual children. Studies in Second Language
Acquisition,
18
(1), 1–25.
Paradis, J., Genesee, F., & Crago, M. B. (Eds.). (2011). Dual
language development and disorders: A handbook on bilingualism and second language
learning. Communication and language intervention series: Vol.
11. Brookes.
Pater, J., & Barlow, J. A. (2003). Constraint
conflict in cluster reduction. Journal of Child
Language,
30
(3), 487–526.
Salameh, E. K., Nettelbladt, U., & Norlin, K. (2003). Assessing
phonologies in bilingual Swedish-Arabic children with and without language impairment. Child
Language Teaching and
Therapy,
19
(3), 338–364.
Schulz, P., & Tracy, R. (2011). LiSe-DaZ –
Linguistische Sprachstandserfassung Deutsch als
Zweitsprache. Hogrefe.
Sorenson Duncan, T., & Paradis, J. (2016). English
language learners’ nonword repetition performance: The influence of age, L2 vocabulary size, length of L2 exposure, and L1
phonology. Journal of Speech, Language, and Hearing
Research,
59
(1), 39–48.
Tamburelli, M., Sanoudaki, E., Jones, G., & Sowinska, M. (2015). Acceleration
in the bilingual acquisition of phonological structure: Evidence from Polish–English bilingual
children. Bilingualism: Language and
Cognition,
18
(4), 713–725.
Tessier, A. M., Sorensen Duncan, T., & Paradis, J. (2013). Developmental
trends and L1 effects in early L2 learners’ onset cluster production. Bilingualism: Language
and
Cognition,
16
(3), 663–681.
van der Hulst, H., & Ritter, N. (2012). Theories
of the syllable. In H. van der Hulst & N. Ritter (Eds.), Studies
in Generative Grammar, Vol. 45. The Syllable: Views and
Facts (pp. 13–52). De Gruyter.
Verhagen, J. & Andringa, S. (2023). The
role of existing language knowledge in bilingual and multilingual toddlers’ repetition of cross-linguistic and
languagespecific nonwords. Linguistic Approaches to
Bilingualism
13
(3), 315–342.
Zembrzuski, D., Marecka, M., Otwinowska, A., Zajbt, E., Krzemiński, M., Szewczyk, J., & Wodniecka, Z. (2020). Bilingual
children do not transfer stress patterns: Evidence from suprasegmental and segmental analysis of L1 and L2 speech of
Polish–English child bilinguals. International Journal of
Bilingualism,
24
(2), 93–114.