References

References

Abrahamsson, N., & Hyltenstam, K.
(2009) Age of onset and nativeness in a second language. Listener perception versus linguistic scrutiny. Language Learning, 59, 249–306. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Flege, J.E., Yeni-Komshian, G.H., & Liu, S.
(1999) Age constraints on second-language acquisition. Journal of Memory and Language, 41, 78–104. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Hakuta, K, Bialystok, E., & Wiley, E.
(2003) Critical evidence: A test of the critical-period Hypothesis for second language acquisition. Psychological Science, 14, 31–38. Crossref[ p. 346 ]Google Scholar
Håkansson, G., & Nettelbladt, U.
(1993) Developmental sequences in L1 (normal and impaired) and L2 acquisition of Swedish syntax. International Journal of Applied Linguistics, 3, 131–157. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Lenneberg, E.
(1967) Biological foundations of language. New York. Wiley.Google Scholar
Mårtensson, J., Eriksson, J., Bodammer, N.C., Lindgren, M., Johansson, M., Nyberg, L., & Lövdén, M.
(2012) Growth of language-related brain areas after foreign language learning. NeuroImage, 63, 240–244. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Rossi, S., Gugler, M. F., Friederici, A. D., & Hahne, A.
(2006) The impact of proficiency on syntactic second-language processing of German and Italian: Evidence from event-related potentials. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 18, 2030–2048. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Tsimpli, I.M.
this volume). Early, late or very late? Timing acquisition and bilingualism. Linguistic Approaches to Bilingualism.
Wexler, K. Schaeffer, J., & Bol, G.
(2004) Verbal syntax and morphology in typically developing Dutch children and children with SLI: developmental data can play an important role in morphological theory. Syntax, 7, 148–198. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Waldmann, C.
(2012) Moving in small steps towards verb second: A case study. Nordic Journal of Linguistics, 34, 331–359. CrossrefGoogle Scholar