References
Abrahamsson, N., & Hyltenstam, K
(2009) Age of onset and nativeness in a second language. Listener perception versus linguistic scrutiny. Language Learning, 591, 249–306. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Flege, J.E., Yeni-Komshian, G.H., & Liu, S
(1999) Age constraints on second-language acquisition. Journal of Memory and Language, 411, 78–104. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Hakuta, K, Bialystok, E., & Wiley, E
(2003) Critical evidence: A test of the critical-period Hypothesis for second language acquisition. Psychological Science, 141, 31–38. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Håkansson, G., & Nettelbladt, U
(1993) Developmental sequences in L1 (normal and impaired) and L2 acquisition of Swedish syntax. International Journal of Applied Linguistics, 31, 131–157. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Lenneberg, E
(1967) Biological foundations of language. New York. Wiley. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Mårtensson, J., Eriksson, J., Bodammer, N.C., Lindgren, M., Johansson, M., Nyberg, L., & Lövdén, M
(2012) Growth of language-related brain areas after foreign language learning. NeuroImage, 631, 240–244. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Rossi, S., Gugler, M. F., Friederici, A. D., & Hahne, A
(2006) The impact of proficiency on syntactic second-language processing of German and Italian: Evidence from event-related potentials. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 181, 2030–2048. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Tsimpli, I.M
this volume). Early, late or very late? Timing acquisition and bilingualism. Linguistic Approaches to Bilingualism.
Wexler, K. Schaeffer, J., & Bol, G
(2004) Verbal syntax and morphology in typically developing Dutch children and children with SLI: developmental data can play an important role in morphological theory. Syntax, 71, 148–198. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Waldmann, C
(2012) Moving in small steps towards verb second: A case study. Nordic Journal of Linguistics, 341, 331–359. DOI logoGoogle Scholar