References
Campbell, A.L., Brooks, P., & Tomasello, M
(2000) Factors affecting young children’s use of pronouns as referring expressions. Journal of Speech, Language, and Hearing Research, 431, 1337–1349. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Cannizzaro, G
(2012) Early word order and animacy. Unpublished doctoral dissertation, University of Groningen.
Chan, A., Lieven, E., & Tomasello, M
(2009) Children’s understanding of the agent–patient relations in the transitive construction: Cross-linguistic comparisons between Cantonese, German, and English. Cognitive Linguistics, 201, 267–300. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Chapman, R.S., & Miller, J.F
(1975) Word order in early two and three word utterances: Does production precede comprehension? Journal of Speech and Hearing Research, 181, 355–371. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Hendriks, P
(2014) Asymmetries between language production and comprehension. Dordrecht: Springer. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
De Hoop, H., & Krämer, I
(2006) Children’s optimal interpretations of indefinite subjects and objects. Language Acquisition, 131, 103–123. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Van Rij, J., van Rijn, H., & Hendriks, P
(2010) Cognitive architectures and language acquisition: A case study in pronoun comprehension. Journal of Child Language, 371, 731–766. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Song, H., & Fisher, C
(2007) Discourse prominence effects on 2.5-year-old children’s interpretation of pronouns. Lingua, 1171, 1959–1987. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Tsimpli, I.M
this volume). Early, late or very late? Timing acquisition and bilingualism. Linguistic Approaches to Bilingualism.
Unsworth, S., Gualmini, A., & Helder, C
(2008) Children’s interpretation of indefinites in sentences containing negation: A reassessment of the cross-linguistic picture. Language Acquisition, 151, 315–328. DOI logoGoogle Scholar