References
Bishop, D.V.M
1994Grammatical errors in specific language impairment: Competence or performance limitations? Applied Psycholinguistics 15: 507–550. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
1997Uncommon Understanding: Development and Disorders of Language Comprehension in Children. Hove: Psychology Press.Google Scholar
Chang, F., Dell, G.S., Bock, J.K. & Griffin, Z.M
2000Structural priming as implicit learning: A comparison of models of sentence production. Journal of Psycholinguistic Research 29: 217–229. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Chang, F
2002Symbolically speaking: A connectionist model of sentence production. Cognitive Science 26: 609–651. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Chomsky, N
1965Aspects of the Theory of Syntax. Cambridge MA: The MIT Press.Google Scholar
1981Lectures on Government and Binding. Dordrecht: Foris.Google Scholar
1986Knowledge of language: Its Nature, Origin and Use. New York NY: Praeger.Google Scholar
1991Some notes on the economy of derivation and representation. In Principles and Parameters in Comparative Grammar, R. Freidin (ed.), 417–454. Cambridge MA: The MIT Press.Google Scholar
1995The Minimalist Program. Cambridge MA: The MIT Press.Google Scholar
1998Minimalist Iniquities: The framework. Cambridge MA: The MIT Press.Google Scholar
Clahsen, H. & Dalalakis, J
1999Tense and agreement in Greek SLI: A case study. Essex Research Reports in Linguistics 24: 1–25.Google Scholar
Diamanti, M
2000Simple morphosyntactic structures in the speech of a child with Specific Language Impairment. In Proceedings of the 8th Symposium of the Panhellenic Association of Logopedist, 201–220. Athens: Ellinika Grammata. (In Greek)Google Scholar
Dick, F., Bates, E., Wulfeck, B., Aydellot, J., Donkers, N. & Gernsbacher, M
2001Language deficits, localization, and grammar: Evidence for a distributive model of language breakdown in aphasic patients and neurologically intact individuals. Psychological Review 108: 759–788. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Dick, F., Wulfeck, B., Krupa-Kwiatkowski, M. & Bates, E
2004The development of complex sentence interpretation in typically developing children compared with children with specific language impairments or early unilateral focal lesions. Developmental Science 7(3): 360–377. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Elman, J.L
1990Finding structure in time. Cognitive Science 14: 179–211. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
1991Distributed representations, Simple Recurrent Networks, and grammatical structure. Machine Learning 7: 195–225. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
1993Learning and development in neural networks: The importance of starting small. Cognition 48: 71–99. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Francis, W. & Kucera, H
1982Frequency Analysis of English Usage. Boston MA: Houghton Miffin.Google Scholar
Fromkin, V., Blair, D. & Collins, P.C
2002An Introduction to Language, 7th edn. Boston MA: Thomson Heinle.Google Scholar
Gathercole, S.E. & Baddeley, A.D
1990Phonological memory deficits in language disordered children: Is there a causal connection? Journal of Memory and Language 29: 336–360. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Gopnik, M
1990Feature-blind grammar and dysphasia. Nature 344 (62–68): 715. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Gopnik, M. & Crago, M
1991Familial aggregation of the developmental language disorders. Cognition 39: 1–50. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Hatzigeorgiou, N., Spiliotopoulou, A., Vacalopoulou, A., Papakostopoulou, A., Piperidis, S., Gavriilidou, M. & Karayanis, G
2000Hellenic National Corpus (HNC): A modern Greek corpus on the internet. In Proceedings of the 21st Annual Meeting of the Linguistics Department, School of Philology, Faculty of Philosophy, Aristotle University of Thessaloniki , 812–821, May 2000, Thessaloniki. (In Greek)
Hoeffner, J.H. & McClelland, J.L
1993Can a perceptual processing deficit explain the impairment of inflectional morphology in developmental dysphasia? A computational investigation. In Proceedings of the 25th Child Language Research Forum, E.V. Clark (ed.), 38–49. Stanford CA: CSLI & Chicago IL: University of Chicago Press.Google Scholar
Holton, D., Mackridge, P. & Philippaki-Warburton, I
2003Greek: An Essential Grammar of the Modern Language. London: Routledge.Google Scholar
Hutzler, F, Ziegler, J.C., Perry, C., Wimmer, H. & Zorzi, M
2004 Do current connectionist learning models account for reading development in different languages? Cognition 91(3): 273–296. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Joanisse, M.F
2004Specific language impairments in children: Phonology, semantics and the English past tense. Current Directions in Psychological Science 13(4): 156–160. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Joanisse, M.F. & Seidenberg M.S
1999Impairments in verb morphology after brain injury: A connectionist model. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America 96: 7592–7597. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Joanisse, M.F. & Seidenberg, M.S
2003Phonology and syntax in specific language impairment: Evidence from a connectionist model. Brain and Language 86: 40–56. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Kail, R
1994A method for studying the generalized slowing hypothesis in children with specific language impairment. Journal of Speech and Hearing Research 37: 418–421. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Kail, M. & Diakogiorgi, K
1994Morphology and word order in the processing of Greek sentences: A crosslinguistic and developmental perspective. In Themes in Greek Linguistics, Current Issues in Linguistic Theory [Current Issues in Linguistics Theory 117], I. Phillipaki-Warburton, K. Nicolaidis & M. Sifianou (eds), 325–332. Amsterdam: John Benjamins. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Karaminis, T
2012Connectionist Modelling of Morphosyntax in Typical and Atypical Development for English and Modern Greek. PhD dissertation, University of London.
Karaminis, T.N. & Thomas M.S.C
2012aConnectionism. In Encyclopedia of the Sciences of Learning, Seel, Norbert M. (ed.), Part 3, 767–771. New York NY: Springer.Google Scholar
Karaminis, T.N. & Thomas, M.S.C
2012bConnectionist theories of learning. In Encyclopedia of the Sciences of Learning, Part 3, N.M. Seel (ed.), 771–774. New York NY: Springer.Google Scholar
Karaminis, T.N., & Thomas, M.S.C
2010A cross-linguistic model of the acquisition of inflectional morphology in English and Modern Greek. In Proceedings of the 32nd Annual Conference of the Cognitive Science Society, S. Ohlsson & R. Catrambone (eds), 730–735. Austin TX: Cognitive Science Society.Google Scholar
Leonard, L.B
1989Language learnability and specific language impairment in children. Applied Psycholinguistics 10: 179–202. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
1998Children with Specific Language Impairment. Cambridge MA: The MIT Press.Google Scholar
Leonard, L.B., Bortolini, U., Caselli, M.C., McGregor, K.K. & Sabbadini, L
1992Morphological deficits in children with specific language impairment: The status of features in the underlying grammar. Language Acquisition 2: 151–179. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Leonard, L.B., McGregor, K.K. & Allen, G
1992Grammatical morphology and speech perception in children with specific language impairment. Journal of Speech and Hearing Research 35: 1076–85. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Lupyan, G. & Christiansen, M.H
2002Case, word order and language learnability: Insights from connectionist modeling. In Proceedings of the 24th Annual Conference of the Cognitive Science Society , 596–601. Mahwah NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.
MacWhinney, B. & Bates, E
1989The Crosslinguistic Study of Sentence Processing. Cambridge: CUP.Google Scholar
Mareschal, D., Johnson, M.H., Sirois, S., Thomas, M.S.C. & Westermann, G
(eds) 2007aNeuroconstructivism, Vol. 1: How the Brain Constructs Cognition. Oxford: OUP. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Mareschal, D., Sirois, S., Westermann, G. & Johnson, M.H
(eds) 2007bNeuroconstructivism, Vol. 2: Perspectives and Prospects. Oxford: OUP. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Marshall, C.R. & van der Lely, H.K
2006A challenge to current models of past tense inflection: The impact of phonotactics. Cognition 100(2): 302–320. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Marslen-Wilson, W.D. & Tyler, L.K
1998Rules, representations, and the English past tense. Trends in Cognitive Sciences 2: 428–435. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Mastropavlou, M
2003aThe Status of gender and agreement features in the grammar of two SLI Children. In Proceedings of the 16th International Symposium of Theoretical and Applied Linguistics, M. Mattheoudakis & A. Psaltou-Joysey (eds), 356–373, Thessaloniki: Aristotle University of Thessaloniki.Google Scholar
2003bSpecific Language Impairment: Description of the speech of three children with SLI and the role of therapy in their development. In Disorders of Communication and Speech: Prevention, Research, Intervention, and New Technologies in Health, M. Glikas & G. Kalomiris (eds), 208–224. Athens: Ellinika Grammata.Google Scholar
Matthews, D.E. & Theakston, A.L
2006Errors of omission in English-speaking children’s production of plurals and the past tense: The effects of frequency, phonology, and competition. Cognitive Science 30: 1027–1052. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Norris, D
2005How do computational models help us build better theories? In Twenty-First Century Psycholinguistics: Four Cornerstones, A. Cutler (ed.), 331–346. Mahwah NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.Google Scholar
Pinker, S
1999Words and Rules. London: Weidenfeld & Nicolson.Google Scholar
Plaut, D.C
2000Methodologies for the computer modeling of human cognitive processes. In Handbook of Neuropsychology, Vol. 1, F. Boller, J. Grafman & G. Rizzotti (eds). Oxford: Elsevier.Google Scholar
Plunkett, K. & Juola, P
1999A connectionist model of English past tense and plural morphology. Cognitive Science 23: 463–490. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Plunkett, K. & Marchman, M
1991U-shaped learning and frequency effects in a multi-layered perceptron: Implications for child language acquisition. Cognition 38(1): 43–102. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Plunkett, K. & Marchman, V
1993From rote learning to system building: Acquiring verb morphology in children and connectionist nets. Cognition 48(1): 21–69. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Rice, M.L
2000Grammatical symptoms of specific language impairment. In Speech and Language Impairments in Children: Causes, Characteristics, Intervention and Outcome, D.V.M. Bishop & L.B. Leonard (eds), 17–34. Hove: Psychology Press.Google Scholar
Rice, M.L., Wexler, K. & Cleave, P
1995Specific Language Impairment as a period of extended optional infinitive. Journal of Speech and Hearing Research 38: 850–863. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Richardson, F.M., Thomas, M.S.C. & Price, C.J
2010Neuronal activation for semantically reversible sentences. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience 22 (5): 943–954. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Rumelhart, D.E., Hinton, G.E. & McClelland, J.L
1986A general framework for parallel distributed processing. In Parallel Distributed Processing: Explorations in the Microstructure of Cognition, Vol. 1: Foundations, D.E. Rumelhart, J.L. McClellandthe PDP Group & (eds), 45–76. Cambridge MA: The MIT Press.Google Scholar
Rumelhart, D.E., Hinton, G. & Williams, R
1986Learning internal representations by error propagation. In Parallel Distributed Processing: Explorations in the Microstructure of Cognition, Vol. 1: Foundations, D.E. Rumelhart, J.L. McClellandthe PDP Group & (eds), 318–362. Cambridge MA: The MIT Press.Google Scholar
Rumelhart, D.E. & McClelland, J.L
1986On learning the past tenses of English verbs. In Parallel Distributed Processing: Explorations in the Microstructure of Cognition, Vol. 2: Psychological and Biological Models, J.L. McClelland & D.E. Rumelhart (eds), 216–271. Cambridge MA: The MIT Press.Google Scholar
Rumelhart, D.E., McClelland, J.Lthe PDP Research Group
& (eds) 1986Parallel Distributed Processing: Explorations in the Microstructure of Cognition, Vol. 1: Foundations. Cambridge MA: The MIT Press.Google Scholar
Seidenberg, M.S
2011Reading in different writing systems: One architecture, multiple solutions. In Dyslexia Across Languages: Orthography and the Gene-Brain-Behavior Link, P. McCardle, J. Ren & O. Tzeng (eds), 146–148. Baltimore MD: Paul H. Brookes.Google Scholar
Smith, N
2008Morphosyntactic Skills and Phonological Short-term Memory in Greek Preschool Children with Specific Language Impairment. PhD dissertation, University of Reading.
Stamouli, S
2000The grammatical categories of tense, aspect, and subject-verb agreement: A comparative study of a Greek-speaking child with specific language impairment and a typical developing child. In Proceedings of the 8th Symposium of the Panhellenic Association of Logopedists , 159–174. Athens: Ellinika Grammata. (In Greek).
Stavrakaki, S
2002Sentence comprehension in Greek SLI children. In Investigations in Clinical Linguistics and Phonetics, N. Hewlett, L. Kelly & F. Windsor (eds), 57–72. Hillsdale NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.Google Scholar
2006Developmental perspectives on Specific Language Impairment: Evidence from the production of wh-questions by Greek SLI children over time. Advances in Speech-Language Pathology 8(4): 384–396. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Stavrakaki, S. & Clahsen, H
2009The perfective past tense in Greek child language. Journal of Child Language 36: 113–142. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Stavrakaki, S., Koutsandreas, K., & Clahsen, H
2012The perfective past tense in Greek children with Specific Language Impairment. Morphology 22: 143–171. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Stephany, U
1997The acquisition of Greek. In The Crosslinguistic Study of Language Acquisition, Vol. 4, D.I. Slobin (ed.), 183–333. Hillsdale NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Assoicates.Google Scholar
Tallal, P. & Piercy, M
1973aDefects of non-verbal auditory perception in children with developmental aphasia. Nature 241: 468–469. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
1973bDevelopmental aphasia: Impaired rate of nonverbal processing as a function of sensory modality. Neuropsychologia 11: 389–398. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Thomas, M.S.C
2005Characterising compensation. Cortex 41(3): 434–442. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Thomas, M.S.C. & Karmiloff-Smith, A
2002Are developmental disorders like cases of adult brain damage? Implications from connectionist modelling. Behavioral and Brain Sciences 25: 727–788. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
2003Modeling language acquisition in atypical phenotypes. Psychological Review 110 (4): 647–682. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Thomas, M.S.C. & Redington, M
2004Modelling atypical syntax processing. In Proceedings of COLING 2004, Psycho-Computational Models of Human Language Acquisition, W.G. Sakas (ed.), 87–94. Geneva: COLING.Google Scholar
Tsimpli, I.M
2001LF-interpretability and language development: A study of verbal and nominal features in Greek normally developing and SLI children. Brain and Language 77: 432–448. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Tzanidaki, D.I
1995Greek word order: Towards a new approach. UCLWorking Paper in Linguistics 7: 247–277.Google Scholar
Ullman, M. & Pierpont, E
2005Specific Language Impairment is not specific to language: The Procedural Deficit hypothesis. Cortex 41(3): 399–433. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
van der Lely, H.K.J
1996Empirical evidence for the modularity of language from Grammatical SLI children. In Proceedings of the 20th Annual Boston University Conference on Language Development, Vol. 2, A. Stringfellow, D. Cahana-Amitay, E. Hughes & A. Zukowski (eds), 781–791. Somerville MA: Cascadilla Press.Google Scholar
van der Lely, H.K.J. & Battell, J
2003Wh-movement in children with grammatical SLI: A test of the RDDR hypothesis. Language 79: 153–181. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
van der Lely, H.K.J. & Ullman, M
2001Past tense morphology in specifically language impaired children and normally developing children. Language and Cognitive Processes 16: 177–217. DOI logoGoogle Scholar