Part of
On the Acquisition of the Syntax of Romance
Edited by Anna Gavarró
[Language Acquisition and Language Disorders 62] 2018
► pp. 3156
References (68)
References
Bencini, G., & Valian, V. (2008). Abstract sentence representations in 3-year olds: Evidence from language production and comprehension. Journal of Memory and Language, 59, 97–113.DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Bever, T. G. (1970). The cognitive basis for linguistic structures. In J. R. Hayes (Ed.), Cognition and the development of language. New York, NY: John Wiley & Sons.Google Scholar
Boeckx, C. (1998). A minimalist view on the passive. University of Connecticut, Papers in Linguistics, Massachusetts Institute of Technology.Google Scholar
Borer, H., & Wexler, K. (1987). The maturation of syntax. In T. Roeper & E. Williams (Eds.), Parameter setting (pp. 123–172). Dordrecht: Reidel.DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Borer, H., & K. Wexler (1992). Bi-unique relations and the maturation of grammatical principles. Natural Language and Linguistic Theory 10, 147–189.DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Bruening, B. (2013). By-phrases in passives and nominals. Syntax, 16(1), 1–41.DOI logoGoogle Scholar
(2014). Word formation is syntactic: Adjectival passives in English. Natural Language & Linguistic Theory, 32(2), 363–422.DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Caprin, C., & Guasti, M. T. (2006). A cross-sectional study on the use of be in early Italian. In V. Torrens & L. Escobar (Eds.), The acquisition of syntax in Romance languages (pp. 117–133). Amsterdam: John Benjamins.DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Chomsky, N. (1995). The minimalist program. Cambridge, MA: The MIT Press.Google Scholar
Collins, C. (2005). A smuggling approach to raising in English. Linguistic Inquiry, 36(2), 289–298.DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Corrêa, L. M. S. (2009). Bootstrapping language acquisition from a minimalist standpoint: On the identification of phi-features in Brazilian Portuguese. In A. Pires & J. Rothman (Eds.), Minimalist inquiries into child and adult language acquisition: Case studies across Portuguese (pp. 35–621). Berlin: Mouton de Gruyter.DOI logoGoogle Scholar
(2014). Interface information and computational cost: An integrated procedural approach to language acquisition with some implications for SLI. In J. Costa, A. Fiéis, M. J. Freitas, M. Lobo, & A. L. Santos (Eds.), New directions in the acquisition of Romance languages. Newcastle upon Tyne: Cambridge Scholars.Google Scholar
Crain, S., Thornton, R., & Murasugi, K. (1987). Capturing the evasive passive. Paper presented at the 12th Boston University Conference on Language Development, Boston.
(2009). Capturing the evasive passive. Language Acquisition, 16(2), 123–133.DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Crawford, J. (2012). Developmental perspectives on the acquisition of the passive (Unpublished doctoral dissertation). University of Connecticut.Google Scholar
Demuth, K. (1989). Maturation and the acquisition of the Sesotho passive. Language, 65, 56–80.DOI logoGoogle Scholar
(1990). Subject, topic and the Sesotho passive. Journal of Child Language, 17, 67–84.DOI logoGoogle Scholar
De Villiers, J. G., & De Villiers, P. A. (1973). Development of the use of word order in comprehension. Journal of Psycholinguistic Research, 2(4), 331–341.DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Diamond, A. (2006). The early development of executive functions. In E. Bialystock & F. I. M. Craik (Eds.), Lifespan cognition. Mechanisms of change (pp. 9970–9995). Oxford: OUP.DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Duarte, I., & Oliveira, F. (2010). Particípios resultativos. InTextos Seleccionados XXV Encontro Nacional da Associação Portuguesa de Linguística. Porto: APL.Google Scholar
Embick, D. (2004). On the structure of resultative participles in English. Linguistic Inquiry, 35(3), 355–392.DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Estrela, A. P. (2013). A aquisição da estrutura passiva em português europeu (Unpublished doctoral dissertation). Universidade Nova de Lisboa.DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Ferreira, F. (2003). The misinterpretation of noncanonical sentences. Cognitive Psychology, 47, 164–203.DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Fox, D., & Grodzinsky, Y. (1998). Children’s passive: A view from the by phrase. Linguistic Inquiry, 29(2), 311–332.DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Gavarró, A., & Parramon, X. (2017). On the adjectival interpretation of passives in acquisition. Lingua, 185, 11–24 DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Gleitman, L. R., January, D., Nappa, R., & Trueswell, J. C. (2007). On the give and take between event apprehension and utterance formulation. Journal of Memory and Language, 57, 544–569.DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Griffin, Z. M., & Bock, K. (2000). What the eyes say about speaking. Psychological Science, 11, 274–279 DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Hirsch, C., & Wexler, K. (2006). Children’s passives and their resulting interpretation. In K. U. Deen, J. Nomura, B. Schulz, & B. D. Schwartz (Eds.), The proceedings of the inaugural conference on generative approaches to language acquisition – North America (pp. 125–136). University of Connecticut Occasional Papers in Linguistics , 4.Google Scholar
Höhle, B., Schmitz, M., Santelmann, L. M., & Weissenborn, J. (2006). The recognition of discontinuous verbal dependencies by German 19-month olds: Evidence for lexical and structural influences on children’s early processing capacities. Language Learning and Development, 2(4), 277–300.DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Hornstein, N., Martins, A. M., & Nunes, J. (2008). Perception and causative structures in English and European Portuguese: Phi-feature agreement and the distribution of bare and prepositional infinitives. Syntax, 11, 198–222.DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Israel, M., Johnson, C., & Brooks, P. J. (2000). From states to events: The acquisition of English passive participles. Cognitive Linguistics, 11, 1–2.Google Scholar
Jusczyk, P. W., & Bertoncini, J. (1988). Viewing the development of speech perception as an innately guided learning process. Language and Speech, 31, 217–238.DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Kline, M., & Demuth, K. (2010). Factors facilitating implicit learning: The case of the Sesotho passive. Language Acquisition, 17(4), 220–234.DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Koring, L., Sangers, N., & Wexler, K. (2015). Who is the agent when it is left implicit? Paper presented at the 12th Generative Approaches to Language Acquisition (GALA) 2015.Google Scholar
Lima Júnior, J. C., & Augusto, M. R. A. (2012). Aspectos estruturais de sentenças passivas relevantes para questões em aquisição da linguagem: uma análise do português. In Anais do VIII ENAL/II EIAL Encontro Inter/Nacional sobre Aquisição da Linguagem em 2011 (pp. 186–199). Juiz de Fora: Programa de Pós-Graduação.Google Scholar
(2014). The relevance of aspectual and semantic features for the comprehension of short passive sentences involving agentive and non-agentive verbs. In J. Costa, A. Fiéis, M. J. Freitas, M. Lobo, & A. L. Santos (Eds.), New directions in the acquisition of Romance languages. Newcastle upon Tyne: Cambridge Scholars.Google Scholar
(2015). Is ‘smuggling’ really necessary? The most recent analyses of passive sentences reconsidered in terms of phasehood and cyclic movement. ReVEL, special edition, 9 < [URL] > Google Scholar
(2017). Passive P and the distinction between eventive, resultative, and stative passives. Diadorim, special edition, 27–52.Google Scholar
Lima Júnior, J. C., & Corrêa, L. M. S. (2015). A natureza do custo computacional na compreensão de passivas: um estudo experimental com adultos. Letras de Hoje, 50(1), 91–101.DOI logoGoogle Scholar
(2016). Sentence-image mapping and the relevance of temporal/aspectual information in the comprehension of passive sentences by children. In M. Carvalho Bezerra Cavalcante et al. (Eds.), Questões em aquisição da linguagem e psicolinguística: fonologia e prosódia, língua de sinais, sintaxe e processamento (pp. 575–589). João Pessoa: Mídia Gráfica e Editora ltda.Google Scholar
(2017). The perception of discontinuous dependencies by 18-month-olds: On the process of acquiring periphrastic verbal passives. Paper presented at the 42nd Boston University Congress of Language Development (BUCLD), Boston.Google Scholar
Manetti, C. (2012). The acquisition of Italian passives: Evidence from comprehension, production and syntactic priming studies (Unpublished doctoral dissertation). University of Siena.Google Scholar
(2013). On the production of passives in Italian: evidence form an elicited production task and a syntactic priming study with preschool children. In S. Bainz, N. Goldman, R. Hawkes (Eds.), BUCLD 37 Proceedings Online Supplement (pp. 1–16).Google Scholar
Maratsos, M., Kuczaj, S. A., Fox, D. C., & Chalkley, M. (1979). Some empirical issues in the acquisition of transformational relations. In W. A. Collins (Ed.), Minnesota symposium on child psychology (Vol. 12, pp. 1–45). Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.Google Scholar
Maratsos, M., Fox, D., Becker, J., & Chalkley, M. A. (1985). Semantic restrictions on children’s passives. Cognition, 56, 271–279.Google Scholar
McIntyre, A. (2012). Adjectival passives and adjectival participles in English. In A. Alexiadou & F. Schaefer (Eds.), Non-canonical passives. Amsterdam: John Benjamins.Google Scholar
Menuzzi, S. (2002). The acquisition of passives in English and Brazilian Portuguese: regularities despite the input In J. Costa & M. J. Freitas (Eds.), Proceedings of Generative Approaches to Language Acquisition (GALA). Lisboa: Associação Portuguesa de Linguística.Google Scholar
Messenger, K., Branigan, H. P., McLean, J. F., & Sorace, A. (2012). Are children’s early passives semantically constrained? Evidence from syntactic priming. Journal of Memory and Language, 66, 568–587.DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Miyake, A., Friedman, N. P., Emerson, M. J., Witzki, A. H., Howerter, A., & Wager, T. D. (2000). The unity and diversity of executive functions and their contributions to complex “frontal lobe” tasks: A latent variable analysis. Cognitive Psychology, 41, 49–100.DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Morgan, J. L., & Demuth, K. (1996). Signal to syntax: An overview. In J. L. Morgan & K. Demuth (Eds.), Signal to syntax: Bootstrapping from speech to grammar in early acquisition (pp. 1–22). Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.Google Scholar
O’Brien, K., Grolla, E., & Lillo-Martin, D. (2006). Long passives are understood by young children. In D. Bamman, T. Magniskaia, & C. Zaller (Eds.), BUCLD 30: Proceedings of the 30th annual Boston University Conference on Language Development (pp. 441–451). Somerville, MA: Cascadilla.Google Scholar
Pinker, S., Lebeaux, D., & Frost, L. (1987). Productivity and constraints in the acquisition of the passive. Cognition, 26, 195–267.DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Richardson, F. M., Thomas, M. S., & Price, C. J. (2010). Neuronal Activation for Semantically Reversible Sentences. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 22(6), 1283–1298.DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Rodrigues, E. S., & Marcilese, M. (2014). Executive control in children’s comprehension of ordinal modifier phrases. In J. Costa, A. Fiéis, M.J. Freitas, M. Lobo, & A. L. Santos (Eds.), New directions in the acquisition of Romance languages. Newcastle upon Tyne: Cambridge Scholars.Google Scholar
Rubin, M. C. B. P. (2009). The passive in 3- and 4-year-olds. Journal of Psycholinguistic Research, 38(5), 435–446.DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Santelmann, L. M., & Jusczyk, P. W. (1998). Sensitivity to discontinuous dependencies in language learners: Evidence for limitations in processing. Cognition, 69, 105–134.DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Slobin, D. (1966). Grammatical transformations and sentence comprehension in childhood and adulthood. Journal of Verbal Learning and Verbal Behavior, 5, 219–227.DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Snyder, W., & Hyams, N. (2015). Minimality effect in children's passives. In E. Di Domenico, C. Hamann, & S. Matteini (Eds.), Structures, strategies and beyond: Studies in honour of Adriana Belletti (pp. 343–368). Amsterdam: John Benjamins.DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Strohner, J., & Nelson, K. E. (1974). The young child’s development of sentence comprehension: Influence of event probability, nonverbal context, syntactic form, and strategies. Child Development, 45, 564–576.DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Sudhalter, V., & Braine, M. (1985). How does comprehension of passive develop? a comparison of actional and experiential verbs. Journal of Child Language, 12, 455–470.DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Terzi, A., & Wexler, K. (2002). A-Chains and s-homophones in children’s grammar: Evidence from Greek passives. Paper presented at NELS 32, 2002.Google Scholar
Tincoff, R., Santelmann, L. M., & Jusczyk, P. W. (2000). Auxiliary verb learning and 18-month-olds’ acquisition of morphological relationships. In S. C. Howell, S. A. Fish & T. Keith-Lucas (Eds.), Proceedings of the 24th annual Boston University Conference on Language Development (pp.726–737). Somerville, MA: Cascadilla Press.Google Scholar
Verrips, M. (1996). Potatoes must peel: The acquisition of the Dutch passive (Unpublished doctoral dissertation). University of Amsterdam.Google Scholar
Volpato, F., Verin, L., Tagliaferro, L., & Cardinaletti, A. (2013). The comprehension of (eventive) verbal passives by Italian preschool age children. In Advances in language acquisition. Proceedings of GALA (Generative Approaches to Language Acquisition) 2011. Newcastle upon Tyne: Cambridge Scholar Publishing.Google Scholar
Volpato, F., Verin, L., & Cardinaletti, A. (2014). The acquisition of passives in Italian: Auxiliaries and answering strategies in an experiment of elicited production. In J. Costa, A. Fiéis, M. J. Freitas, M. Lobo, & A. L. Santos (Eds.), New directions in the acquisition of Romance languages. Newcastle upon Tyne: Cambridge Scholars.Google Scholar
Wasow, T. (1977). Transformations and the lexicon. In P. Culicover, T. Wasow, & J. Bresnan (Eds.), Formal syntax (pp. 327–360). New York, NY: Academic Press.Google Scholar
Wexler, K. (2002). Lenneberg’s dream: Learning, normal language development and specific language impairment. In J. Schaeffer & Y. Levy (Eds.), Language competence across populations: Towards a definition of specific language impairment (pp. 11–61). Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.Google Scholar
(2004). Theory of phasal development: Perfection in child grammar. MIT Working Papers in Linguistics, 48, 159–209.Google Scholar
Cited by (1)

Cited by one other publication

Correia, Liliana, Maria Lobo & Cristina Flores
2024. Sentence repetition task for European Portuguese: results from a study with monolingual and Portuguese-German bilingual children. Language Acquisition  pp. 1 ff. DOI logo

This list is based on CrossRef data as of 27 july 2024. Please note that it may not be complete. Sources presented here have been supplied by the respective publishers. Any errors therein should be reported to them.