References
Åfarli, T. A., & Eide, K. M.
(2003) Norsk generativ syntaks. Oslo: Novus Forlag.Google Scholar
Alsina, A.
(1992) On the argument structure of causatives. Linguistic Inquiry, 23(4), 517–555.Google Scholar
Baltin, M.
(1995) Floating quantifiers, PRO and predication. Linguistic Inquiry, 26(2), 199–248.Google Scholar
Boeckx, C., Hornstein, N., & Nunes, J.
(2010) Control as movement. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Carnie, A.
(2013) Syntax: A generative introduction (3rd ed.). Chichester: John Wiley & Sons.Google Scholar
Chomsky, N.
(1982) Some concepts and consequences of the theory of government and binding. Cambridge, MA: The MIT Press.Google Scholar
(1993) Lectures on government and binding: The Pisa lectures. Berlin: Mouton de Gruyter. (Original work published 1981). DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Chomsky, N., & Lasnik, H.
(1993) The theory of principles and parameters. In J. Jacobs, A. von Stechow, W. Sternefeld & T. Vennemann (Eds.), Syntax: An international handbook of contemporary research (Vol. 1, pp. 506–569). Berlin: Walter de Gruyter.Google Scholar
Dixon, R. M. W., & Aikhenvald, A. Y.
(Eds.) (2000) Changing valency: Case studies in transitivity. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Faarlund, J. T.
(2007) Parameterization and change in non-finite complementation. Diachronica, 24(1), 57–80. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
(2015) The Norwegian infinitive marker. Working Papers in Scandinavian Syntax, 95, 1–10.Google Scholar
Faarlund, J. T., Lie, S., & Vannebo, K. I.
(1997) Norsk referansegrammatikk (Norwegian reference grammar). Oslo: Universitetsforlaget.Google Scholar
Hestvik, A., Nordby, H., & Karlsen, G.
(2005) Antecedent reactivation by surface and deep anaphora in Norwegian. Scandinavian Journal of Psychology, 46(3), 229–38. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Hestvik, A., Schwartz, R. G., & Tornyova, L.
(2010) Relative clause gap-filling in children with Specific Language Impairment. Journal of Psycholinguistic Research, 39(5), 443–456. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Hornstein, N.
(1999) Movement and Control. Linguistic Inquiry, 30(1), 69– 96. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Hornstein, N., & Nunes, J.
(2014) Minimalism and Control. In A. Carnie, D. Siddiqi, & Y. Sato (Eds.), Routledge handbook of syntax (pp. 239–263). Abingdon: Routledge. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Janke, V.
(2003) A PRO-less theory of Control. In A. Neeleman & R. Vermeulen (Eds.), University College London Working Papers in Linguistics (UCLWPL) (Vol. 15, pp. 213–242). London: UCL.Google Scholar
(2008) Control without a subject. Lingua, 118(1), 82–118. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Kuznetsova, A., Brockhoff, P. B., & Christensen, R. H. B.
(2016) lmerTest: Tests in linear mixed effects models. R package version 2.0-32.Google Scholar
Larsen, T. N.
(2017)  Control and raising: Gone with(out) a trace? (Unpublished MA thesis). The University of Bergen.
Lødrup, H.
(2008) Raising to object in Norwegian and the derived object constraint. Studia Linguistica, 62(2), 155–181. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Manzini, M. R., & Roussou, A.
(2000) A minimalist theory of A-movement and control. Lingua, 110(6), 409–447. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Martin, R.
(2001) Null case and the distribution of PRO. Linguistic Inquiry, 32(1), 141–166. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Neeleman, A., & van de Koot, H.
(2002) The configurational matrix. Linguistic Inquiry, 33(4), 529–574. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Nicol, J.
(1988)  Coreference processing during sentence comprehension (Unpublished doctoral dissertation). MIT.
Nicol, J., & Osterhout, L.
(1988) Reactivating antecedents of empty categories during parsing (Unpublished manuscript). University of Arizona, Tucson.
Nicol, J., & Swinney, D.
(1989) The role of structure in coreference assignment during sentence comprehension. Journal of Psycholinguistic Research, 18(1), 5–19. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Postal, P.
(1974) On raising: One rule of English grammar and its theoretical implications. Cambridge, MA: The MIT Press.Google Scholar
Radford, A.
(2004) Minimalist syntax: Exploring the structure of English. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Reed, L.
(2014) Strengthening the PRO hypothesis. Berlin: Mouton de Gruyter.Google Scholar
Rosenbaum, P.
(1967) The grammar of English predicate complement constructions. Cambridge, MA: The MIT Press.Google Scholar
Schumacher, P. B., Dangl, M., & Uzun, E.
(2016) Thematic role as prominence cue during pronoun resolution in German. In A. Holler & K. Suckow (Eds.), Empirical perspectives on anaphora resolution (pp. 121–147). Berlin: Walter de Gruyter.Google Scholar
Sigurðsson, H.
(2008) The case of PRO. Natural Language & Linguistic Theory, 26(2), 403–450. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Walenski, M.
(2002)  Relating parsers and grammars: On the structure and real-time comprehension of English infinitival complements (Unpublished doctoral dissertation). University of California, San Diego.
Williams, E.
(1980) Predication. Linguistic Inquiry, 11(1), 203–238.Google Scholar
Cited by

Cited by 1 other publications

Larsen, Tori & Christer Johansson
2022. Control in a Norwegian grammar maze. Nordic Journal of Linguistics  pp. 1 ff. DOI logo

This list is based on CrossRef data as of 23 april 2024. Please note that it may not be complete. Sources presented here have been supplied by the respective publishers. Any errors therein should be reported to them.