Chapter published in:
Typical and Impaired Processing in Morphosyntax
Edited by Vincent Torrens
[Language Acquisition and Language Disorders 64] 2020
► pp. 279301
References

References

Abbeduto, L., Murphy, M. M., Cawthon, S. W., Richmond, E. K., Weissman, M. D., Karadottir, S., & O’Brien, A.
(2003) Receptive language skills of adolescents and young adults with Down syndrome or fragile X syndrome. American Journal on Mental Retardation, 108(3), 149–160. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Abbeduto, L., Warren, S. F., & Conners, F. A.
(2007) Language development in Down syndrome: From the prelinguistic period to the acquisition of literacy. Mental Retardation and Developmental Disabilities Research Reviews, 13(3), 247–261. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Abbot-Smith, K., & Behrens, H.
(2006) How known constructions influence the acquisition of other constructions: The German passive and future constructions. Cognitive Science, 30(6), 995–1026. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Aktas, M.
(2004)  Sprachentwicklungsdiagnostik bei Kindern mit Down- Syndrom. Entwicklung eines diagnostischen Leitfadens zum theoriegeleiteten Einsatz standardisierter Verfahren (Unpublished doctoral dissertation). University of Bielefeld. Retrieved from https://​pub​.uni​-bielefeld​.de​/publication​/2302157 (4 May, 2020).
Aschermann, E., Gülzow, I., & Wendt, D.
(2004) Differences in the comprehension of passive voice in German-and English-speaking children. Swiss Journal of Psychology, 63(4), 235–245. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Baddeley, A.
(1990) Human memory. Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.Google Scholar
Baddeley, A., & Jarrold, C.
(2007) Working memory and Down syndrome. Journal of Intellectual Disability Research, 51(Pt 12), 925–931. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Baddeley, A., Gathercole, S., & Papagno, C.
(1998) The phonological loop as a language learning device. Psychological Review, 105(1), 158–173. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Bartke, S.
(2004) Passives in German children with Williams syndrome. In S. Bartke & J. Siegmüller (Eds.), Williams syndrome across languages (pp. 345–370). Amsterdam: John Benjamins. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Bever, T. G.
(1970) The cognitive basis for linguistic structures. In R. Hayes (Ed.), Cognition and language development (pp. 279–362). New York, NY: Wiley & Sons.Google Scholar
Bridges, A., & Smith, J. V.
(1984) Syntactic comprehension in Down’s syndrome children. British Journal of Psychology, 75(2), 187–196. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Caplan, D., Baker, C., & Dehaut, F.
(1985) Syntactic determinants of sentence comprehension in aphasia. Cognition, 21, 117–175. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Chapman, R. S., Hesketh, L. J., & Kistler, D. J.
(2002) Predicting longitudinal change in language production and comprehension in individuals with Down syndrome: Hierarchical linear modeling. Journal of Speech, Language, and Hearing Research, 45(5), 902–915. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Chapman, R. S., Schwartz, S. E., & Bird, E. K.
(1991) Language skills of children and adolescents with Down syndrome, I: Comprehension. Journal of Speech and Hearing Research, 34(5), 1106–1120. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Chomsky, N.
(1981) Lectures on government and binding. Dordrecht: Foris.Google Scholar
Clahsen, H., Kursawe, C., & Penke, M.
(1996) Introducing CP: Wh-questions and subordinate clauses in German child language. In C. Koster & F. Wijnen (Eds.), Proceedings of the Groningen Assembly on Language Acquisition (pp. 5–22). Groningen: University of Groningen, Center for Language and Cognition.Google Scholar
Eisenbeiss, S.
(1993) Auxiliaries and the acquisition of the passive. In E. V. Clark (ed.), The Proceedings of the 25th Annual Child Language Research Forum (pp. 235–242). Stanford, CA: CSLI.Google Scholar
Eriks-Brophy, A., Goodluck, H., & Stojanovic
(2004) Comprehension and production of syntax in high-functioning individuals with Down syndrome. In M-O. Junker, M. McGinnis, & Y. Roberge (Eds.), Proceedings of the 2004 annual conference of the Canadian Linguistic Association. Montreal (Canada).Google Scholar
Fowler, A. E.
(1990) Language abilities in children with Down syndrome: Evidence for a specific syntactic delay. In D. Cicchetti & M. Beeghly (Eds.), Children with Down syndrome (pp. 302–328). Cambridge: Cambridge University Press. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Fox, A. V.
(2011) TROG-D Test zur Überprüfung des Grammatikverständnisses. Idstein: Schulz-Kirchner.Google Scholar
Grimm, H.
(2003) Sprachscreening für das Vorschulalter (SSV) [Language screening for preschool children]. Göttingen: Hogrefe.Google Scholar
Grodzinsky, Y.
(1995) A restrictive theory of agrammatic comprehension. Brain and Language, 50, 27–51. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Haegeman, L.
(1994) Introduction to government and binding theory. Oxford: Blackwell.Google Scholar
Joffe, V., & Varlokosta, S.
(2007) Patterns of syntactic development in children with Williams syndrome and Down’s syndrome: Evidence from passives and wh-questions. Clinical Linguistics & Phonetics, 21(9), 705–727. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Laws, G., & Gunn, D.
(2004) Phonological memory as a predictor of language comprehension in Down syndrome: A five-year follow-up study. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 45(2), 326–337. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Miolo, G., Chapman, R. S., & Sindberg, H. A.
(2005) Sentence comprehension in adolescents with Down syndrome and typically developing children: Role of sentence voice, visual context, and auditory-verbal short-term memory. Journal of Speech, Language, and Hearing Research, 48(1), 172–188. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Næss, K.-A. B., Lyster, S.-A. H., Hulme, C., & Melby-Lervåg, M.
(2011) Language and verbal short-term memory skills in children with Down syndrome: A meta-analytic review. Research in Developmental Disabilities, 32(6), 2225–2234. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Penke, M.
(2015) Syntax and language disorders. In T. Kiss & A. Alexiadou (Eds.), Syntax – Theory and analysis. An international handbook (Vol. 3, pp. 1833–1874). Berlin: De Gruyter Mouton. Google Scholar
Penner, Z., & Kölliker Funk, M.
(1998) Therapie und Diagnose von Grammatikerwerbsstörungen: Ein Arbeitsbuch. Luzern: SZH/SPC.Google Scholar
Perovic, A.
(2006) Syntactic deficit in Down syndrome: More evidence for the modular organisation of language. Lingua, 116, 1616–1630. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Ring, M., & Clahsen, H.
(2005) Distinct patterns of language impairment in Down’s syndrome and Williams syndrome: The case of syntactic chains. Journal of Neurolinguistics, 18(6), 479–501. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Roberts, J. E., Chapman, R. S., Martin, G. E., & Moskowitz, L.
(2008) Language of preschool and school-age children with Down syndrome and fragile X syndrome. In J. E. Roberts, R. S. Chapman, & S. F. Warren (Eds.), Speech and language development and intervention in Down syndrome and fragile X syndrome (pp. 77–115). Baltimore, MD: Paul H. Brookes.Google Scholar
Roesch, A. D., & Chondrogianni, V.
(2015) The use of case in the comprehension of wh-questions in German-speaking children with and without SLI. In C. Hamann & E. Ruigendijk (Eds.), Language acquisition and development: Proceedings of GALA 2013 (pp. 379–402). Newcastle upon Tyne: Cambridge Scholars.Google Scholar
Rubin, M.
(2006) The passive in adolescents with Down syndrome: A case study. Down’s Syndrome, Research and Practice, 11(2), 88–96. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Sanoudaki, E., & Varlokosta, S.
(2015) Pronoun comprehension in individuals with Down syndrome: The role of age. International Journal of Language & Communication Disorders / Royal College of Speech & Language Therapists, 50(2), 176–186. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Schaner-Wolless, C., Binder H., & Tamchina, D.
(1986) Frühes Leid mit der Leideform. Zum Passiverwerb des Deutschens. Wiener Linguistische Gazette, 37, 5–38.Google Scholar
Schlesewsky, M., Fanselow, G., Kliegl, R., & Krems, J.
(2000) The subject preference in the processing of locally ambiguous wh-questions in German. In B. Hemforth & L. Konieczny (Eds.), German sentence processing (pp. 65–93). Dordrecht: Springer. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Siegmüller, J., Herzog, C., & Herrmann, H.
(2005) Syntaktische und lexikalische Aspekte beim Verstehen von Informationsfragen – Ein Vergleich zwischen Kindern mit spezifischer Sprachentwicklungsstörung und Kindern mit ungestörter Sprachentwicklung. L.O.G.O.S. Interdisziplinär, 13, 29–35.Google Scholar
Siegmüller, J., Kauschke, C., van Minnen, S., & Bittner, D.
(2011) Test zum Satzverstehen von Kindern (TSVK): Eine profilorientierte Diagnostik der Syntax. München: Elsevier.Google Scholar
Tellegen, P. J., Laros, J. A., & Petermann, F.
(2007) SON-R 2½-7. Non-verbaler Intelligenztest [SON-R 2½-7. Snijders-Oomen Nonverbal Intelligence Test] (Revised 2nd ed. with German norms). Göttingen: Hogrefe.Google Scholar
Thordardottir, E. T., Chapman, R. S., & Wagner, L.
(2002) Complex sentence production by adolescents with Down syndrome. Applied Psycholinguistics, 23(02), 163–183. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Tsakiridou, M.
(2006) The linguistic profile of Down’s syndrome subjects: evidence from Wh-movement construction. SOAS Working Papers in Linguistics, 14, 227–248.Google Scholar
Vallar, G., & Baddeley, A.
(1984) Fractionation of working memory: Neuropsychological evidence for a phonological short-term store. Journal of Verbal Learning and Verbal Behavior, 23(2), 151–61. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Van der Lely, H. K.
(1996) The Test of Active and Passive Sentences (TAPS). London: DLDCN.Google Scholar
Wimmer, E., Rothweiler, M., & Penke, M.
(2017) Acquisition of who-question comprehension in German children with hearing loss. Journal of Communication Disorders, 67, 35–48. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Witecy, B., & Penke, M.
(2017) Language comprehension in children, adolescents, and adults with Down syndrome. Research in Developmental Disabilities, 62, 184–196. CrossrefGoogle Scholar