References
Bever, T. G., & McElree, B.
(1988) Empty categories access their antecedents during comprehension. Linguistic Inquiry, 19, 35–43.Google Scholar
Cuetos, F., & Mitchell, D. C.
(1988) Crosslinguistic differences in parsing. Cognition, 30, 73–105. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Gordon, P. C., Hendrick, R., & Johnson, M.
(2004) Effects of noun phrase type on sentence complexity. Journal of Memory and Language, 51, 97–114. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Nicol, J., & Swinney, D.
(1989) The role of structure in coreference assignment during sentence comprehension. Journal of Psycholinguistic Research, 18(1):5–19. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Torrens, V.
(Ed.) (2018) Identification of children with Specific Language Impairment. Language Acquisition, 25(1), 1–117. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Townsend, D. J., & Bever, T. G.
(2001) Sentence comprehension: The integration of habits and rules. Cambridge, MA: The MIT Press. DOI logoGoogle Scholar