Part of
L1 Acquisition and L2 Learning: The view from Romance
Edited by Larisa Avram, Anca Sevcenco and Veronica Tomescu
[Language Acquisition and Language Disorders 65] 2021
► pp. 3966
References (55)
References
Adani, F., Sehm, M., & Zukowski, A. (2012). How do German children and adults deal with their relatives. In S. Stavrakaki, M. Lalioti, & P. Konstantinopoulou (Eds.), Advances in language acquisition (pp. 14–22). Cambridge Scholars.Google Scholar
Adani, F., van der Lely, H. K. J., Forgiarini, M., & Guasti, M. T. (2010). Grammatical feature dissimilarities make RCs easier: A comprehension study with Italian children. Lingua, 120, 2148–2166. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Augusto, M. R., Rodrigues, E. dos S., & Costa, I. (2020). “Topic Subject” structure and agreement with weather verbs within relative clauses: Experimental evidence from adult and children Brazilian Portuguese speakers. In A. Cardinaletti, C. Branchini, G. Giusti, & F. Volpato (Eds.), Language acquisition, processing and bilingualism: Selected papers from the Romance Paper VII (pp. 241–262). Cambridge Scholars.Google Scholar
Avelar, J. & Galves, C. (2011). Tópico e concordância em PB e PE [Topic and Agreement in Brazilian Portuguese and in European Portuguese]. In J. Costa, P. Barbosa, & I. Falé (Eds.), XXVI Encontro Nacional da Associação Portuguesa de Linguística. Textos selecionados (pp. 49–65). APL.Google Scholar
Baayen, R. H., Davidson, D. J., & Bates, D. M. (2008). Mixed-effects modeling with crossed random effects for subjects and items. Journal of Memory and Language, 59, 390–412. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Bates, D., Maechler, M., Bolker, B., & Walker, S. (2015). Fitting linear mixed-effects models using lme4. Journal of Statistical Software, 67(1), 1–48. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Belletti, A., & Contemori, C. (2010). Intervention and attraction. On the production of subject and object relatives by Italian (young) children and adults. In J. Costa, A. Castro, M. Lobo, & F. Pratas (Eds.), Language acquisition and development: Proceedings of GALA 2009 (pp. 39–52). Cambridge Scholars.Google Scholar
(2012). Subjects in children’s object relatives in Italian. Revue Roumaine de Linguistique, LVII(2), 117–142.Google Scholar
Belletti, A., Friedmann, N., Brunato, D., & Rizzi, L. (2012). Does gender make a difference? Comparing the effect of gender on children’s comprehension of RCs in Hebrew and Italian. Lingua, 122, 1053–1069. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Bențea, A. (2012). Subject vs. object relatives: What can Romanian children tell us about their acquisition? Revue Roumaine de Linguistique, LVII(2), 203–218.Google Scholar
Bențea, A., Durrleman, S., & Rizzi, L. (2016). Refining intervention: The acquisition of featural relations in object A-bar dependencies. Lingua, 169, 21–41. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Cabral, A. F. V., Leitão, M. M., & Kenedy, E. (2015). A influência da animacidade no processamento das cláusulas relativas em português brasileiro [The influence of animacy on relative clause processing in Brazilian Portuguese]. Letras de Hoje, 50, 102–111. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Corrêa, L. M. S. (1995). An alternative assessment of children’s comprehension of relative clauses. Journal of Psycholinguistic Research, 24(3), 183–203. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Costa, I., Augusto, M. R. A., & Rodrigues, E. dos S. (2014). Verbos meteorológicos flexionados no plural e a hipótese da inacusatividade biargumental: explorando a sintaxe do Português Brasileiro [Weather verbs in the plural and the unnacusativity hypothesis: Aspects of Brazilian Portuguese syntax]. Veredas (UFJF. Online), 18, 257–280.Google Scholar
Costa, J., Friedmann, N., Silva, C., & Yachini, M. (2014). The boy that the chef cooked. Acquisition of PP relatives in European Portuguese and Hebrew. Lingua, 150, 386–409. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
(2015). The acquisition of PP relatives in Hebrew and European Portuguese: Another window into the atoms of intervention. In C. Hamann & E. Ruigendijk (Eds.), Language acquisition and development: Proceedings of GALA 2013 (pp. 35–48). Cambridge Scholars.Google Scholar
De Villiers, J., Tager-Flusberg, H., Hakuta, K., & Cohen, M. (1979). Children’s comprehension of relative clauses. Journal of Psycholinguistic Research, 8, 499–518. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
De Villiers, P. (1988). Assessing English syntax in hearing-impaired children: Elicited production in pragmatically motivated situations. Journal of the Academy of Rehabilitative Audiology: Monograph Supplement 21, 41–71.Google Scholar
Fadlon, J., Morgan, A. M., Meltzer-Asscher, A., & Ferreira, V. S. (2019). It depends: Optionality in the production of filler-gap dependencies. Journal of Memory and Language, 106, 40–76. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Ferreira, V. S. (1996). Is it better to give than to donate? Syntactic flexibility in language production. Journal of Memory and Language, 35, 724–755. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Ferreiro, E., Othenin-Girard, C., Chipman, H., & Sinclair, H. (1976). How do children handle relative clauses? Archives de Psychologie, 45, 229–266.Google Scholar
Friedmann, N., Belletti, A., & Rizzi, L. (2009). Relativized relatives. Types of intervention in the acquisition of A-Bar dependencies. Lingua, 119(1), 67–88. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Gibson, E., Desmet, T., Grodner, D., Watson, D., & Ko, K. (2005). Reading relative clauses in English. Cognitive Linguistics, 16(2), 313–353. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Goodluck, H. & Tavakolian, S. (1982). Competence and processing in children’s grammar of relative clauses. Cognition, 11(1), 1–27. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Gordon, P. C., Hendrick, R., & Johnson, M. (2001). Memory interference during language processing. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 27(6), 1411–1423.Google Scholar
(2004). Effects of noun phrase type on sentence complexity. Journal of Memory and Language, 51(1), 97–114. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Gordon, P. C., Hendrick, R., & Levine, W. H. (2002). Memory-load interference in syntactic processing. Psychological Science, 13(5), 425–430. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Grolla, E., & Augusto, M. (2016). Absolutive constructions in Brazilian Portuguese and Relativized Minimality effects in children’s productions. In L. Perkins, R. Dudley, J. Gerard, & K. Hitczenko (Eds.), Proceedings of GALANA VI – Generative Approaches to Language Acquisition North America (pp. 36–47). Cascadilla Press.Google Scholar
Grolla, E., Rodrigues, E., & Augusto, M. (in preparation). Differential demands in production and comprehension of relative clauses by children.
Guasti, M. T., & Cardinaletti, A. (2003). Relative clause formation in Romance child production. Probus, 15(1), 47–88. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Hamburger, H. & Crain, S. (1982). Relative acquisition. In S. Kuczaj (Ed.), Language development: Syntax and semantics (pp. 245–274). Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.Google Scholar
Holmberg, A. (2010). Null subject parameters. In T. Biberauer, A. Holmberg, I. Roberts, & M. Sheehan (Eds.), Parametric variation: Null subjects in minimalist theory (pp. 88–124). Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Hornstein, N. (2001). Move! A Minimalist Theory of Construal. Blackwell.Google Scholar
Hwang, H., & Kaiser, E. (2014). Having a syntactic choice is not always better: the effects of syntactic flexibility on Korean production. Language, Cognition and Neuroscience, 29(9), 1115–1131. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Kato, M. A., & Nunes, J. (2009). A uniform raising analysis for standard and nonstandard relative clauses in Brazilian Portuguese. In J. Nunes. (Ed.) Minimalist essays on Brazilian Portuguese syntax (pp. 93–120). John Benjamins. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Keenan, E. L., & Comrie, B. (1977). Noun phrase accessibility and Universal Grammar. Linguistic Inquiry, 8(1), 63–99.Google Scholar
Kidd, E., Brandt, S., Lieven, E., & Tomasello, M. (2007). Object relatives made easy: A cross-linguistic comparison of the constraints influencing young children’s processing of RCs. Language and Cognitive Processes, 22(6), 860–897. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Labelle, M. (1988). Predication et mouvement: Le developpement de la relative chez les enfants Francophones (Unpublished doctoral dissertation). University of Ottawa.
(1990). Predication, wh-movement and the development of relative clauses. Language Acquisition, 1(1), 95–119. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Lobo, M., & Vaz, S. (2017). Does the animacy of the antecedent play a role in the production of relative clauses? Matraga, 24(41), 266–287. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Mak, W., Vonk, W., & Schriefers, H. (2002). The influence of animacy on relative clause processing. Journal of Memory and Language, 47, 50–68. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Myachykov, A., Scheepers, C., Garrod, S., Thompson, D., & Fedorova, O. (2013). Syntactic flexibility and competition in sentence production: The case of English and Russian. The Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 66(8), 1601–1619. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Pérez-Leroux, A. T. (1995). Resumptives in the acquisition of relative clauses. Language Acquisition, 4, 105–138. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
R Core Team. (2018). R: A language and environment for statistical computing. Vienna, Austria: R Foundation for Statistical Computing. Retrieved from [URL]
Rangel, M. (2017). O traço de animacidade e as estratégias de relativização em português brasileiro infantil: Um estudo experimental [The animacy feature and the relativization strategies in child Brazilian Portuguese: An experimental study] (Unpublished master thesis). Universidade de São Paulo.
Rispoli, M., & Hadley, P. (2001). The leading edge: The significance of sentence disruptions in the development of grammar. Journal of Speech, Language, and Hearing Research, 44, 1131–1143. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Rizzi, L. (1990). Relativized Minimality. The MIT Press.Google Scholar
Rouveret, A. (2011). Introduction. In A. Rouveret (Ed.), Resumptive pronouns at the interfaces (pp. 1–62). John Benjamins. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Schad, D., Hohenstein, S., Vasishth, S., & Kliegl, R. (2019). How to capitalize on a priori contrasts in linear (mixed) modes: A tutorial. arXiv:1807.10451v4 [stat.ME] 17 Jul 2019. Retrieved from: [URL]
Sevcenco, A., & Avram, L. (2012). Romanian-speaking children’s comprehension of relatives. Revue Roumaine de Linguistique, LVII(2), 219–239.Google Scholar
Traxler, M. J., Morris, R. K., & Seely, R. E. (2002). Processing subject and object relative clauses: Evidence from eye movements. Journal of Memory & Language, 47(1), 69–90. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Utzeri, I. (2007). The production and the acquisition of subject and object relative clauses in Italian: A comparative experimental study. Nanzan Linguistics (Special Issue 3). Papers from the Consortium Workshops on Linguistic Theory. 2006–2007, 1, 283–313.Google Scholar
Vasilyeva, M., & Waterfall, H. (2012). Beyond syntactic priming: Evidence for activation of alternative syntactic structures. Journal of Child Language, 39(2), 258–283. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Wanner, E., & Maratsos, M. (1978). An ATN approach to comprehension. In M. Halle, J. Bresnan, & G. A. Miller (Eds.), Linguistic theory and psychological reality (pp. 119–161). The MIT Press.Google Scholar
Warren, T., & Gibson, E. (2002). The influence of referential processing on sentence complexity. Cognition, 85, 79–112. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Cited by (1)

Cited by one other publication

Lira, Thainá Amador & Marina Augusto
2024. Chapter 6. Relative clauses and intervention effects. In Language Acquisition in Romance Languages [Bilingual Processing and Acquisition, 18],  pp. 146 ff. DOI logo

This list is based on CrossRef data as of 10 august 2024. Please note that it may not be complete. Sources presented here have been supplied by the respective publishers. Any errors therein should be reported to them.