Part of
L1 Acquisition and L2 Learning: The view from Romance
Edited by Larisa Avram, Anca Sevcenco and Veronica Tomescu
[Language Acquisition and Language Disorders 65] 2021
► pp. 315330
References
Barr, D. J.
(2013) Random effects structure for testing interactions in linear mixed-effects models. Frontiers in Psychology, 4, 328. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Barr, D. J., Levy, R., Scheepers, C., & Tily, H. J.
(2013) Random effects structure for confirmatory hypothesis testing: Keep it maximal. Journal of Memory and Language, 68(3), 255–278. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Chierchia, G.
(2017) Scalar implicatures and their interface with grammar. Annual Review of Linguistics, 3, 245–264. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Crain, S.
(2012) Emergence of meaning. Cambridge University Press. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Crain, S., Ni, W-J., & Conway, L.
(1994) Learning, parsing and modularity. In C. Clifton, L. Frazier, & K. Rayner (Eds.), Perspectives on sentence processing (pp. 443–467). Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.Google Scholar
Geçkin, V., Crain, S., & Thornton, R.
(2015) The interpretation of logical connectives in Turkish. Journal of Child Language, 43(4), 784–810. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Geçkin, V., Thornton, R., & Crain, S.
(2017) Children’s interpretation of disjunction in negative sentences: A comparison of Turkish and German. Language Acquisition, 25(2), 197–212. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Giannakidou, A.
(2006) N-words and negative concord. In M. Everaert & H. van Riemsdijk (Eds.), The Blackwell companion to syntax (Vol. 3, pp. 327–391). Wiley Online Library. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Giannakidou, A., & Zeijlstra, H.
(2017) The landscape of negative dependencies: Negative concord and n-words. In The Wiley Blackwell companion to syntax (2nd ed., pp. 1–38). Wiley-Blackwell. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Goro, T.
(2007) Language-specific constraints on scope interpretation in first language acquisition (Unpublished PhD dissertation). University of Maryland.
Goro, T., & Akiba, S.
(2004a) Japanese disjunction and the acquisition of positive polarity. In Y. Otsu (Ed.), Proceeding of the 5th Tokyo Conference on Psycholinguistics (pp. 137–162). Hituzi Shobo.Google Scholar
(2004b) The acquisition of disjunction and positive polarity in Japanese. In V. Chand, A. Kelleher, A. J. Rodriguez, & B. Schmeiser (Eds.), Proceedings of the 23rd West Coast Conference on Formal Linguistics (pp. 251–264). Cascadilla Press.Google Scholar
Pagliarini, E., Crain, S., & Guasti, M. T.
(2018) The compositionality of logical connectives in child Italian. Journal of Psycholinguistic Research, 47, 1243–1277. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Pagliarini, E., Lungu, O., van Hout, A., Pinter, L., Surányi, B., Crain, S., & Guasti, M. T.
in press). How children interpret disjunction under negation in Dutch, French, Hungarian and Italian: A cross-linguistic comparison. Language, Learning and Development.
Pagliarini, E., Reyes, M. A., Guasti, M. T., Crain, S., & Gavarró, A.
(2021) Negative sentences with disjunction in child Catalan. Language Acquisition, 28(2), 153–165. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Shimada, H., & Goro, T.
(2021) On the source of children’s conjunctive interpretation of disjunction: Scope, strengthening, or both? Language Acquisition, 28(2), 98–130. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Spector, B.
(2014) Global positive polarity items and obligatory exhaustivity. Semantics and Pragmatics, 7(11), 1–61. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Szabolcsi, A.
(2002) Hungarian disjunctions and positive polarity. Approaches to Hungarian, 8, 1–22.Google Scholar
World Medical Association
(2013) World Medical Association Declaration of Helsinki. Ethical principles for medical research involving human participants. JAMA Journal of the American Medical Association, 310(20), 2191–2194. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Zanuttini, R.
(1997) Negation and clausal structure: A comparative study of Romance languages. Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Zeijlstra, H. H.
(2004) Sentential negation and negative concord. LOT/ACLC.Google Scholar