References (60)
References
Abe, J. (2015). The in-situ approach to sluicing. John Benjamins. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Adams, P. W., & Tomioka, S. (2012). Sluicing in Mandarin Chinese: An instance of pseudo-sluicing. In J. Merchant & A. Simpson (Eds.), Sluicing: Cross-linguistic perspectives (pp. 219–247). Oxford University Press. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Adani, F., van der Lely, K. J., Forgiarini, M., & Guasti, M. T. (2010). Grammatical feature dissimilarities make relative clauses easier: A comprehension study with Italian children. Lingua, 120, 2148–2166. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Biondo, N., Pagliarini, E., Moscati, V., Rizzi, L., & Belletti, A. (2022). Features matter: The role of number and gender features during the online processing of subject- and object-relative clauses in Italian. Language, Cognition and Neuroscience, 38(6), 802–820. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Chung, S., Ladusaw, W. A., & McCloskey, J, (1995). Sluicing and logical form. Natural Language Semantics, 3, 239–282. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Clahsen, H., & Felser, C. (2006a). Grammatical processing in language learners. Applied Psycholinguistics, 27, 3–42. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
(2006b). Continuity and shallow structures in language processing. Applied Psycholinguistics, 27, 107–126. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
(2017). Some notes on the shallow structure hypothesis. Studies in Second Language Acquisition, 40, 693–706. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Culicover, P., & Jackendoff, R. (2005). Simpler syntax. Oxford University Press. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Cunnings, I. (2017). Parsing and working memory in bilingual sentence processing. Bilingualism: Language and Cognition, 20, 659–678. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Cunnings, I., & Fujita, H. (2021). Similarity-based interference and relative clauses in second language processing. Second Language Research, 39(2). DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Durrleman, S., & Bentea, A. (2021). Locality in the acquisition of object A’-dependences: Insights from French. Glossa: A Journal of General Linguistics, 6, 106. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Franciotti, P., & Martohardjono, G. (2022). Intervention effects in L1 and L2 English raising: Evidence from acceptability judgment and response times. In T. Leal, E. Shimanskaya, & C. A. Isabelli (Eds.), Generative SLA in the age of minimalism: Features, interfaces, and beyond (pp. 11–40). John Benjamins. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Friedmann, N., Belletti, A., & Rizzi, L. (2009). Relativized relatives: Types of intervention in the acquisition of A-bar dependencies. Lingua, 119, 67–88. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Fujiwara, Y. (2020). Sprouting: A key to unifying Japanese sluicing. University of Pennsylvania Working Papers in Linguistics, 26, 87–96.Google Scholar
Ginzburg, J., & Sag, I. (2000). Interrogative investigations. CSLI.Google Scholar
Gordon, P. C., Hendrick, R., Johnson, M., & Lee, Y. (2006). Similarity-based interference during comprehension: Evidence from eye tracking during reading. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 32, 1304–1321. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Gordon, P. C., Hendrick, R., & Levine, W. H. (2002). Memory-load interference in syntactic processing. Psychological Science, 13, 425–430. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Goto, K., Maki, H., & Kasai, C. (2010). The minimal English test: A new method to measure English as a second language proficiency. Evaluation & Research in Education, 23, 91–104. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Hasebe, M. (2014). The acquisition of the wh-movement operation in English by Japanese EFL learners (Unpublished doctoral dissertation). Yokohama National University.
Hiraiwa, K. (2021). Sluicing cannot apply in-situ in Japanese. Proceedings of the Linguistics Society of America, 6, 317–324. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Hiraiwa K., & Ishihara, S. (2012). Syntactic metamorphosis: Cleft, sluicing, and in-situ focus in Japanese. Syntax, 15, 142–180. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Hopp, H. (2016). The timing of lexical and syntactic processes in second language sentence comprehension. Applied Psycholinguistics, 37, 1253–1280. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
(2017). The processing of English which-questions in adult L2 learners: Effects of L1 transfer and proficiency. Zeitschrift für Sprachwissenschaft, 36, 107–134. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Hu, S., Gavarró, A., Vernice, M., & Guasti, M. T. (2016). The acquisition of Chinese relative clauses: Contrasting two theoretical approaches. Journal of Child Language, 43, 1–21. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Jäger, G. (2001). Indefinites and sluicing: A type of logical approach. In M. Stokhof & R. van Rooij (Eds.), Proceedings of the 13th Amsterdam colloquium (pp. 114–119). ILLC.Google Scholar
JASP Team (2020). JASP (Version 0.13.1) [Computer software].Google Scholar
Kimura, H. (2010). A wh-in-situ strategy for sluicing. English Linguistics, 27, 43–59. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Kimura, T. (2022). Explaining the difficulty with scope interpretations by speakers of a scope-rigid language. In T. Leal, E. Shimanskaya, & C. A. Isabelli (Eds.), Generative SLA in the age of minimalism; Features, interfaces, and beyond (pp. 41–66). John Benjamins. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Kizu, M. (2005). Cleft constructions in Japanese syntax. Palgrave Macmillan. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Kuwabara, K. (1996). Multiple wh-phrase in elliptical clauses and some aspects of clefts with multiple foci. In M. Koizumi, M. Oishi, & U. Sauerland (Eds.), Proceedings of FAJL2: Formal Approaches to Japanese Linguistics (pp. 97–116). MITWPL.Google Scholar
Lasnik, H. (2014). Multiple sluicing in English? Syntax, 17, 1–20. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Lewis, R. L., Vasishth, S., & Van Dyke, J. A. (2006). Computational principles of working memory in sentence comprehension. Trends in Cognitive Science, 10, 447–454. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Lindenbergh C., van Hout, A., & Hollebrandse, B. (2015). Extending ellipsis research: The acquisition of sluicing in Dutch. In E. Grillo, K. Jepson, & M. LaMendola (Eds.), BUCLD 39 Online proceedings supplement of the 32nd Boston University Conference on Language Development. Retrieved on 16 May 2024 from [URL]
Liu, M. (2019). The syntax and acquisition of Mandarin sluice-like constructions (Unpublished MA thesis). UCLA.Google Scholar
Liu, M., Hyams, N., & Mateu, V. (2020). Late intervention effects in the acquisition of Mandarin sluice-like constructions. In M. M. Brown, & A. Kohut (Eds.), Proceedings of the 44th annual Boston University Conference on Language Development (pp. 322–335). Cascadilla Press.Google Scholar
Maki, H., Kasai, C., Goto, K., Ito T., Miyamoto, Y., & Oku, S. (2007). The minimal English test: Its correlation with the university entrance examination (English Part) 2006. Bulletin of the Faculty of Regional Studies, 21, 127–134.Google Scholar
Martini, K. (2019). Animacy does not help French-speaking children in the repetition of object relatives. In P. Guijarro-Fuentes & C. Suárez-Gómez (Eds.), Proceedings of GALA 2017: Language acquisition and development (pp. 221–240). Cambridge Scholars.Google Scholar
Mateu, V. (2020). Intervention effects in the acquisition of raising: Evidence from English and Spanish. Language Acquisition, 27, 1–34. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Mateu, V., & Hyams, N. (2020). Structural intervention effects in the acquisition of sluicing. Language Acquisition, 28, 6–38. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Merchant, J. (2001). The syntax of silence. Oxford University Press. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Miura, A. (2020). Validating the intervention effect in L2 English acquisition: From on-line and off-line experiments (Unpublished MA thesis). Kanagawa University.
Ohba, A., & Schwartz, B. D. (2022). Sluicing in the L2 English of L1 Japanese speakers. In Y. Gong & F. Kpogo (Eds.), Proceedings of the 46th annual Boston University Conference on Language Development (pp. 586–599). Cascadilla Press.Google Scholar
Ohba, A. Shimada, H., & Yamakoshi, K. (2018). The structure of sluicing and the availability of strict and sloppy readings in child Japanese. In A. B. Bertolini & M. J. Kaplan (Eds.), Proceedings of the 42nd annual Boston University Conference on Language Development (pp. 605–617). Cascadilla Press.Google Scholar
Rizzi, L. (1990). Relativized minimality. The MIT Press.Google Scholar
(2004). Locality and left periphery. In A. Belletti (Ed.), Structures and beyond. The cartography of syntactic structure (Vol. 3, pp. 223–251). Oxford University Press. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
(2013). Locality. Lingua, 130, 169–186. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Ross, J. R. (1969). Guess who? In R. Binnick, A. Davison, G. Green, & J. Morgan (Eds.), Proceedings of the Fifth Regional Meeting of the Chicago Linguistic Society (pp. 252–86). CLS.Google Scholar
Saito, M. (2004). Ellipsis and pronominal reference in Japanese clefts. Nanzan Linguistics, 1, 21–50.Google Scholar
Street, J. A. (2017). This is the native speaker that the non-native speaker outperformed: Individual, education-related differences in the processing and interpretation of object relative clauses by native and non-native speakers of English. Language Sciences, 59, 192–203. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Sugisaki, K. (2016). Sluicing and its identity condition in the acquisition of Japanese. In J. Scott & D. Waughtal (Eds.), Proceedings of the 40th annual Boston University Conference on Language Development (pp. 346–359). Cascadilla Press.Google Scholar
Suzuki, K. (2019). Intervention effect in relative clause production in L2 English. Poster presented at Generative Approaches to Second Language Acquisition (GASLA XV), Reno, March 2019.
Takahashi, D. (1994). Sluicing in Japanese. Journal of east Asian linguistics, 3, 265–300. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Van Craenenbroeck, J. & Lipták, A. (2013). What sluicing can do, what it can’t, and in which language: On the cross-linguistic syntax of ellipsis. In L. L. Cheng & N. Corver (Eds.), Diagnosing syntax (pp. 502–536). Oxford University Press. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Van Dyke, J. A., & MacElree, B. (2006). Retrieval interference in sentence comprehension. Journal of Memory and Language, 55, 157–166. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
(2011). Cue-dependent interference in comprehension. Journal of Memory and Language, 65, 243–263. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Villata, S., Rizzi, L., & Franck, J. (2016). Intervention effects and Relativized Minimality: New experimental evidence from graded judgements. Lingua, 179, 76–96. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
White, L. (1989). Universal Grammar and second language acquisition. John Benjamins. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Wood, G. C. (2009). The identity of silence: Acquiring the identity condition on sluicing. In M. Sztencel (Ed.), Newcastle working papers in linguistics (Vol. 15, pp. 138–150). Newcastle University Press.Google Scholar
Xia, V. Y., White, L., & Guzzo, N. B., (2022). Intervention in relative clauses: Effects of relativized minimality on L2 representation and processing. Second Language Research, 38, 343–372. DOI logoGoogle Scholar