References (38)
Abbot-Smith, K., & Behrens, H.
(2006) How known constructions influence the acquisition of other constructions: The German passive and future constructions. Cognitive Science, 30(6), 995–1026. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Bader, M., & Meng, M.
(2018) The misinterpretation of noncanonical sentences revisited. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 44(8), 1286–1311. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Barr, D. J., Levy, R., Scheepers, C., & Tily, H. J.
(2013) Random effects structure for confirmatory hypothesis testing: Keep it maximal. Journal of Memory and Language, 68(3), 255–278. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Borer, H., & Wexler, K.
(1987) The maturation of syntax. In T. Roeper & E. Williams (Eds.), Parameter setting, (pp. 123–172). Reidel. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Boyce, V., Futrell, R., & Levy, R. P.
(2020) Maze made easy: Better and easier measurement of incremental processing difficulty. Journal of Memory and Language, 111, 104082. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Carrithers, C.
(1989) Syntactic complexity does not necessarily make sentences harder to understand. Journal of Psycholinguistic Research, 18(1), 75–88. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Christianson, K., Hollingworth, A., Halliwell, J. F., & Ferreira, F.
(2001) Thematic roles assigned along the garden path linger. Cognitive Psychology, 42(4), 368–407. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Christianson, K., Luke, S. G., & Ferreira, F.
(2010) Effects of plausibility on structural priming. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 36(2), 538–544. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Clahsen, H., & Felser, C.
(2006a) Grammatical processing in language learners. Applied Psycholinguistics, 27(1), 3–42. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
(2006b) Continuity and shallow structures in language processing. Applied Psycholinguistics, 27(1), 107–126. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
(2006c) How native-like is non-native language processing? Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 10(12), 564–570. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
(2018) Some notes on the Shallow Structure Hypothesis. Studies in Second Language Acquisition, 40(3), 693–706. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Drummond, A., von der Malsburg, T., Erlewine, M. Y., & Vafaie, M.
(2016) Ibex farm. Retrieved on 16 May 2024 from [URL]
Felser, C., Roberts, L., Marinis, T., & Gross, R.
(2003) The processing of ambiguous sentences by first and second language learners of English. Applied Psycholinguistics, 24(3), 453–489. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Ferreira, F.
(2003) The misinterpretation of noncanonical sentences. Cognitive Psychology, 47(2), 164–203. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Ferreira, F., Bailey, K. G. D., & Ferraro, V.
(2002) Good-enough representations in language comprehension. Current Directions in Psychological Science, 11(1), 11–15. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Ferreira, F., & Christianson, K.
(2016) Is now-or-never language processing good enough? Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 39. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Ferreira, F., & Patson, N. D.
(2007) The “good enough” approach to language comprehension. Language and Linguistics Compass, 1(1–2), 71–83. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Gehrke, B., & Grillo, N.
(2007) Aspects on passives. In S. Blaho, E. Schoorlemmer, & V. Luis (Eds.) Proceedings of ConSOLE XIV (pp. 121–141). Student Organisation of Linguistics in Europe.Google Scholar
(2009) How to become passive. In B. Gehrke & N. Grillo, Explorations of Phase Theory: Features and arguments (pp. 231–268) Mouton de Gruyter. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Gelman, A., & Hill, J.
(2007) Data analysis using regression and multilevel/hierarchical models. Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Grillo, N., Alexiadou, A., Gehrke, B., Hirsch, N., Paolazzi, C., & Santi, A.
(2019) Processing unambiguous verbal passives in German. Journal of Linguistics, 55(3), 523–562. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Gulordava, K., Bojanowski, P., Grave, E., Linzen, T., & Baroni, M.
(2018) Colorless green recurrent networks dream hierarchically. In M. Walker, H. Ji, & A. Stent (Eds.), Proceedings from the 2018 Conference of the North American Chapter of the Association for Computational Linguistics: Human Technologies (Vol.1). ACL. DOI logo
Hale, J.
(2001) A probabilistic Earley parser as a psycholinguistic model. In L. Levin (Ed.), Proceedings of the Second Meeting of the North American Chapter of the Association for Computational Linguistics (pp. 159–166). ACL. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Jacob, G., & Felser, C.
(2016) Reanalysis and semantic persistence in native and non-native garden-path recovery. Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 69(5), 907–925. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Karimi, H., & Ferreira, F.
(2016) Good-enough linguistic representations and online cognitive equilibrium in language processing. Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 69(5), 1013–1040. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Levy, R.
(2008) Expectation-based syntactic comprehension. Cognition, 106(3), 1126–1177. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Meng, M., & Bader, M.
(2020) Does comprehension (sometimes) go wrong for noncanonical sentences? Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 74(1), 1–28. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Osterhout, L., & Swinney, D. A.
(1993) On the temporal course of gap-filling during comprehension of verbal passives. Journal of Psycholinguistic Research, 22(2), 273–286. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Oxford University Press, University of Cambridge, & Association of Language Testers in Europe
(2001) Quick placement test: Paper and pen test. Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Paolazzi, C. L., Grillo, N., Alexiadou, A., & Santi, A.
(2019) Passives are not hard to interpret but hard to remember: Evidence from online and offline studies. Language, Cognition and Neuroscience, 34(8), 991–1015. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Paolazzi, C. L., Grillo, N., Cera, C., Karageorgou, F., Bullman, E., Chow, W. Y., & Santi, A.
(2021) Eyetracking while reading passives: An event structure account of difficulty. Language, Cognition and Neuroscience, 37(2), 135–153. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Papadopoulou, D., & Clahsen, H.
(2003) Parsing strategies in L1 and L2 sentence processing: A study of relative clause attachment in Greek. Studies in Second Language Acquisition, 25(4), 501–528. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
RStudio Team
(2021) RStudio: Integrated Development for R. RStudio, PBC, Boston, MA. [URL]
Roberts, L., & Felser, C.
(2011) Plausibility and recovery from garden paths in second language sentence processing. Applied Psycholinguistics, 32(2), 299–331. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Street, J. A., & Dąbrowska, E.
(2010) More individual differences in language attainment: How much do adult native speakers of English know about passives and quantifiers? Lingua, 120(8), 2080–2094. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Traxler, M. J., Corina, D. P., Morford, J. P., Hafer, S., & Hoversten, L. J.
(2013) Deaf readers’ response to syntactic complexity: Evidence from self-paced reading. Memory & Cognition, 42(1), 97–111. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Witzel, N., Witzel, J., & Forster, K.
(2011) Comparisons of online reading paradigms: Eye tracking, moving-window, and maze. Journal of Psycholinguistic Research, 41(2), 105–128. DOI logoGoogle Scholar