Article published In:
Language and Linguistics
Vol. 20:4 (2019) ► pp.569601
References (73)
Altmann, Heidi
2006The perception and production of second language stress: A cross-linguistic experimental study. Newark: University of Delaware. (Doctoral dissertation.)Google Scholar
Altmann, Heidi & Vogel, Irene
2002L2 acquisition of stress: The role of L1. (Paper presented at the DGfS Annual Meeting, Mannheim, 27 February-1 March 2002.)
Archibald, John
1997The acquisition of English stress by speakers of nonaccentual languages: Lexical storage versus computation of stress. Linguistics 35(1). 167–181. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Baayen, R. Harald & Piepenbrock, Richard & van Rijn, Hedderik
1993The CELEX database (CD-ROM). Philadelphia: Linguistic Data Consortium.Google Scholar
Bates, Douglas & Mächler, Martin & Bolker, Ben & Walker, Steve
2015Fitting linear mixed-effects models using lme4. Journal of Statistical Software 67(1). 1–48. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Best, Catherine T.
1994The emergence of native-language phonological influence in infants: A perceptual assimilation model. In Goodman, Judith C. & Nusbaum, Howard C. (eds.), The transition from speech sounds to spoken words: The development of speech perception, 167–224. Cambridge: The MIT Press.Google Scholar
Boersma, Paul & Weenink, David
2016Praat: Doing phonetics by computer (Version 6.0.14). ([URL]) (Accessed 2016-02-11).
Bond, Zinny S. & Small, Larry H.
1983Voicing, vowel, and stress mispronunciations in continuous speech. Perception & Psychophysics 34(5). 470–474. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Booij, Geert E.
1995The phonology of Dutch. Oxford: Clarendon Press.Google Scholar
Broersma, Mirjam & Cutler, Anne
2008Phantom word activation in L2. System 36(1). 22–34. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
2011Competition dynamics of second-language listening. The Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology 64(1). 74–95. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Chao, Yuen-ren
1968A grammar of spoken Chinese. Berkeley: University of California Press.Google Scholar
Cheng, Chin-chuan
1968English stresses and Chinese tones in Chinese sentences. Phonetica 18(2). 77–88. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Chomsky, Noam & Halle, Morris
1968The sound pattern of English. New York: Harper & Row.Google Scholar
Chrabaszcz, Anna & Winn, Matthew B. & Lin, Candise Y. & Idsardi, William J.
2014Acoustic cues to perception of word stress by English, Mandarin, and Russian speakers. Journal of Speech, Language, and Hearing Research 57(4). 1468–1479. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Connine, Cynthia M. & Blasko, Dawn G. & Wang, Jian
1994Vertical similarity in spoken word recognition: Multiple lexical activation, individual differences, and the role of sentence context. Perception and Psychophysics 56(6). 624–636. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Cooper, Nicole & Cutler, Anne & Wales, Roger
2002Constraints of lexical stress on lexical access in English: Evidence from native and non-native listeners. Language and Speech 45(3). 207–228. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Crowther, Court S. & Mann, Virginia
1992Native language factors affecting use of vocalic cues to final consonant voicing in English. The Journal of the Acoustical Society of America 92(2). 711–722. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Cutler, Anne
1986Forbear is a homophone: Lexical prosody does not constrain lexical access. Language and Speech 29(3). 201–220. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Cutler, Anne & Butterfield, Sally
1992Rhythmic cues to speech segmentation: Evidence from juncture misperception. Journal of Memory and Language 31(2). 218–236. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Cutler, Anne & Carter, David M.
1987The predominance of strong initial syllables in the English vocabulary. Computer Speech & Language 2(3). 133–142. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Cutler, Anne & Chen, Hsuan-chih
1997Lexical tone in Cantonese spoken-word processing. Perception and Psychophysics 59(2). 165–179. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Cutler, Anne & Norris, Dennis
1988The role of strong syllables in segmentation for lexical access. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance 14(1). 113–121.Google Scholar
Cutler, Anne & Otake, Takashi
2004Pseudo-homophony in non-native listening. The Journal of the Acoustical Society of America 115(5). 2392. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Cutler, Anne & Pasveer, Dennis
2006Explaining cross-linguistic differences in effects of lexical stress on spoken-word recognition. In Hoffman, Rüdiger & Mixdorff, Hansjörg (eds.), Proceedings of the Third International Conference on Speech Prosody, 250–254. Dresden: TUD press.Google Scholar
Cutler, Anne & Wales, Roger & Cooper, Nicole & Janssen, Joris
2007Dutch listeners’ use of suprasegmental cues to English stress. In Barry, William J. & Trouvain, Jürgen (eds.), Proceedings of the 16th International Congress of Phonetic Sciences (ICPhS XVI), 1913–1916. Saarbrücken: Universität des Saarlandes.Google Scholar
Cutler, Anne & Weber, Andrea & Otake, Takashi
2006Asymmetric mapping from phonetic to lexical representations in second-language listening. Journal of Phonetics 34(2). 269–284. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Dupoux, Emmanuel & Pallier, Christophe & Sebastián-Gallés, Núria & Mehler, Jacques
1997A destressing “deafness” in French? Journal of Memory and Language 36(3). 406–421. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Dupoux, Emmanuel & Sebastián-Gallés, Núria & Navarrete, Eduardo & Peperkamp, Sharon
2008Persistent stress “deafness”: The case of French learners of Spanish. Cognition 106(2). 682–706. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Fear, Beverley D. & Cutler, Anne & Butterfield, Sally
1995The strong/weak syllable distinction in English. The Journal of the Acoustical Society of America 97(3). 1893–1904. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Flege, James E.
1995Second language speech learning: Theory, findings, and problems. In Strange, Winifred (ed.), Speech perception and linguistic experience: Issues in cross-language research, 233–277. Timonium: York Press.Google Scholar
Fry, Dennis B.
1955Duration and intensity as physical correlates of linguistic stress. The Journal of the Acoustical Society of America 27(4). 765–768. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Gaskell, M. Gareth & Marslen-Wilson, William D.
2002Representation and competition in the perception of spoken words. Cognitive Psychology 45(2). 220–266. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Gay, Thomas
1978Physiological and acoustic correlates of perceived stress. Language and Speech 21(4). 347–353. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Goldinger, Stephen D. & Luce, Paul A. & Pisoni, David B.
1989Priming lexical neighbors of spoken words: Effects of competition and inhibition. Journal of Memory and Language 28(5). 501–518. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Iverson, Paul & Kuhl, Patricia K. & Akahane-Yamada, Reiko & Diesch, Eugen & Tohkura, Yoh’ich & Kettermann, Andreas & Siebert, Claudia
2003A perceptual interference account of acquisition difficulties for non-native phonemes. Cognition 87(1). B47–B57. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Juffs, Alan
1990Tone, syllable structure and interlanguage phonology: Chinese learners’ stress errors. International Review of Applied Linguistics in Language Teaching 28(2). 99–117. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Kochanski, Greg & Orphanidou, Christina
2008What marks the beat of speech? The Journal of the Acoustical Society of America 123(5). 2780–2791. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Lee, Chao-yang
2007Does horse activate mother? Processing lexical tone in form priming. Language and Speech 50(1). 101–123. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Lehiste, Ilse
1972The timing of utterances and linguistic boundaries. The Journal of the Acoustical Society of America 51(6B). 2018–2024. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Luce, R. Duncan
1986Response times: Their role in inferring elementary mental organization. New York: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Lukyanchenko, Anna & Idsardi, William J. & Jiang, Nan
2011Opening your ears: The role of L1 in processing of nonnative prosodic contrasts. In Granena, Gisela & Koeth, Joel & Lee-Ellis, Sunyoung & Lukyanchenko, Anna & Prieto Botana, Goretti & Rhoades, Elizabeth (eds.), Selected proceedings of the Second Language Research Forum 2010, 50–62. Somerville: Cascadilla Press.Google Scholar
Marslen-Wilson, William D.
1980Speech understanding as a psychological process. In Simon, Jean-Claude (ed.), Spoken Language Generation and Understanding, 39–67. Dordrecht: D. Reidel. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
1990Activation, competition, and frequency in lexical access. In Altmann, Gerry T. M. (ed.), Cognitive models of speech processing: Psycholinguistic and computational perspectives, 148–172. Cambridge: The MIT Press.Google Scholar
Marslen-Wilson, William D. & Warren, Paul
1994Levels of perceptual representation and process in lexical access: Words, phonemes, and features. Psychological Review 101(4). 653–675. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Marslen-Wilson, William D. & Zwitserlood, Pienie
1989Accessing spoken words: The importance of word onsets. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance 15(3). 576–585.Google Scholar
McQueen, James M. & Norris, Dennis & Cutler, Anne
1999Lexical influence in phonetic decision making: Evidence from subcategorical mismatches. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance 25(5). 1363–1389.Google Scholar
Nakatani, Lloyd H. & Dukes, Kathleen D.
1977Locus of segmental cues for word juncture. The Journal of the Acoustical Society of America 62(3). 714–719. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Norris, Dennis & McQueen, James M. & Cutler, Anne
1995Competition and segmentation in spoken-word recognition. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition 21(5). 1209–1228.Google Scholar
Ou, Shu-chen
2010Taiwanese EFL learners’ perception of English word stress. Concentric: Studies in Linguistics 36(1). 1–23.Google Scholar
2016Perception of English lexical stress with a marked pitch accent by native speakers of Mandarin. Taiwan Journal of Linguistics 14(2). 1–31.Google Scholar
Peperkamp, Sharon
1997Prosodic words (HIL Dissertations 34). The Hague: Holland Academic Graphics.Google Scholar
Peperkamp, Sharon & Dupoux, Emmanuel
2002A typological study of stress “deafness”. Laboratory Phonology 71. 203–240.Google Scholar
Psychology Software Tools
2012E-Prime 2.0. (Computer Software) Pittsburgh: Author.Google Scholar
R Core Team
2015R: A language and environment for statistical computing. Vienna: R Foundation for Statistical Computing. ([URL]).
Rastle, Kathleen & Harrington, Jonathan & Coltheart, Max
2002358, 534 nonwords: The ARC nonword database. The Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology: Section A 55(4). 1339–1362. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Schaefer, Vance & Darcy, Isabelle
2014Lexical function of pitch in the first language shapes cross-linguistic perception of Thai tones. Laboratory Phonology 5(4). 489–522. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Slowiaczek, Louisa M.
1990Effects of lexical stress in auditory word recognition. Language and Speech 33(1). 47–68. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Small, Larry H. & Simon, Stephen D. & Goldberg, Jill S.
1988Lexical stress and lexical access: Homographs and non-homographs. Perception and Psychophysics 44(3). 272–280. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
So, Connie K. & Best, Catherine T.
2010Cross-language perception of non-native tonal contrasts: Effects of native phonological and phonetic influences. Language and Speech 53(2). 273–293. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Soto-Faraco, Salvador & Sebastián-Gallés, Núria & Cutler, Anne
2001Segmental and suprasegmental mismatch in lexical access. Journal of Memory and Language 45(3). 412–432. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Swinney, David A.
1979Lexical access during sentence comprehension: (Re)consideration of context effects. Journal of Verbal Learning and Verbal Behavior 18(6). 645–659. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Takagi, Naoyuki
1995Signal detection modeling of Japanese listeners’ /r/–/l/ labeling behavior in a one-interval identification task. The Journal of the Acoustical Society of America 97(1). 563–574. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Tremblay, Annie
2008Is second language lexical access prosodically constrained? Processing of word stress by French Canadian second language learners of English. Applied Psycholinguistics 29(4). 553–584. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
van Donselaar, Wilma & Koster, Mariëtte & Cutler, Anne
2005Exploring the role of lexical stress in lexical recognition. The Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology Section A 58(2). 251–273. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Van Heuven, Vincent J.
1985Perception of stress pattern and word recognition: Recognition of Dutch words with incorrect stress position. Journal of the Acoustical Society of America 781. S21. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Van Leyden, Klaske & van Heuven, Vincent J.
1996Lexical stress and spoken word recognition: Dutch vs. English. Linguistics in the Netherlands 13(1). 159–170. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Vitevitch, Michael S.
2007The spread of the phonological neighborhood influences spoken word recognition. Memory & Cognition 35(1). 166–175. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Vroomen, Jean & de Gelder, Béatrice
1995Metrical segmentation and lexical inhibition in spoken word recognition. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance 21(1). 98–108.Google Scholar
Wang, Qian
2008aPerception of English stress by Mandarin Chinese learners of English: An acoustic study. Victoria: University of Victoria. (Doctoral dissertation.)Google Scholar
2008bL2 stress perception: The reliance on different acoustic cues. In Barbosa, Plinio A. & Madureira, Sandra & Reis, César (eds.), Proceedings of the 4th International Conference on Speech Prosody, 635–638.Google Scholar
Woodward, Julide Z. & Aslin, Richard N.
1990Segmentation cues in maternal speech to infants. (Paper presented at the 7th biennial meeting of the International Conference on Infant Studies, Montreal, April 1990.)
Zwitserlood, Pienie
1989The locus of the effects of sentential-semantic context in spoken-word processing. Cognition 32(1). 25–64. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Cited by (2)

Cited by 2 other publications

Guo, Chunfeng & Kapil Sharma
2022. Intelligent Voice System Design for Optimizing E-Business Advertising Rhetoric Based on SVM Algorithm. Computational Intelligence and Neuroscience 2022  pp. 1 ff. DOI logo
Ma, Hongjuan & Sang-Bing Tsai
2021. The Technique of English Word Syllable Division in Speech Synthesis Based on Neural Network. Mathematical Problems in Engineering 2021  pp. 1 ff. DOI logo

This list is based on CrossRef data as of 5 july 2024. Please note that it may not be complete. Sources presented here have been supplied by the respective publishers. Any errors therein should be reported to them.