Article published in:
Transitivity and Valency: From theory to acquisition
Edited by Georgia Fotiadou and Hélène Vassiliadou
[Lingvisticæ Investigationes 40:1] 2017
► pp. 117133
References

References

Abbot-Smith, K., Lieven, E., & Tomasello, M.
(2004) Training 2;6-year-olds to produce the transitive construction: The role of frequency, semantic similarity and shared syntactic distribution. Developmental Science, 7(1), 48–55. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
[ p. 130 ]
Bencini, G. M. L., & Valian, V. V.
(2008) Abstract sentence representations in 3-year-olds: Evidence from language production and comprehension. Journal of Memory and Language, 59(1), 97–113. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Bock, J. K., & Griffin, Z. M.
(2000) The persistence of structural priming: Transient activation or implicit learning? Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 129(2), 177–192. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Bourmayan, A.
(2013) Les objets implicites en français : une approche pragmatique. Doctoral dissertation. École des hautes études en sciences sociales.Google Scholar
Cardinaletti, A., & Starke, M.
(1999) The typology of structural deficiency: A case study of the three classes of pronouns. In H. Van Riemsdijk (Ed.), Clitics in the languages of Europe (pp. 145–233). Berlin: Mouton de Gruyter.Google Scholar
Childers, J., & Tomasello, M.
(2001) The role of pronouns in young children’s acquisition of the English transitive construction. Developmental Psychology, 37, 739–748. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Cohen, J.
(1988) Statistical Power Analysis for the Behavioral Sciences (second ed.). Hillsdale, N.J.: L. Erlbaum Associates.Google Scholar
Cummins, S., & Roberge, Y.
(2005) A modular account of null objects in French. Syntax, 8, 44–64. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Déchaine, R., & Wiltschko, M.
(2002) Decomposing pronouns. Linguistic Inquiry, 33, 409–442. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Gleitman, L. R.
(1990) The structural sources of verb meanings. Language Acquisition, 1, 3–55. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Göksun, T., Küntay, A. & Naigles, L.
(2008) Turkish children use morphosyntax in extending verb meaning. Journal of Child Language, 35, 291–323. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Hamann, C., Rizzi, L. & Frauenfelder, U.
(1996) The acquisition of subject and object clitics in French. In H. Clahsen (Ed.), Generative perspectives on language acquisition (pp. 309–334). Amsterdam: John Benjamins. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Ingham, R.
(1993/1994) Input and learnability: direct object omissibility in English. Language Acquisition, 3, 95–120. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Jakubowicz, C., Müller, N., Kang, O. -K., Riemer, B. & Rigaut, C.
(1996) On the acquisition of the pronominal system in French and German. In A. Springfellow, D. Cahana-Amitay, E. Hughes & A. Zukowski (Eds.), Proceedings of the 20th Boston University Conference in Language Development (pp. 374–385). Somerville, MA: Cascadilla Press.Google Scholar
Kayne, R. S.
(1975) French syntax: The transformational cycle. Cambridge: MIT Press.Google Scholar
Kline, M. & Demuth, K.
(2014) Syntactic generalizations with novel intransitive verbs. Journal of Child Language, 41(3), 543–74. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Lambrecht, K. & Lemoine, K.
(1996) Vers une grammaire des compléments zéro en français parlé, in J. Chuquet & M. Frid (Eds.), Absence de marques et représentation de l’absence (pp. 279–309). Rennes: Presses universitaires de Rennes.Google Scholar
(2005) Definite null objects in (spoken) French. A construction-grammar account, in M. Fried & H. Boas (Eds.), Proceedings of the First International Construction-Grammar Conference (pp. 13–55). Amsterdam, Philadelphia: Benjamins.Google Scholar
Larjavaara, M.
(2000) Présence ou absence de l’objet. Limites du possible en français contemporain. Helsinki: Academia Scientiarum Fennica.Google Scholar
Lee, J. & Naigles, L. R.
(2005) Input to verb learning in Mandarin Chinese: A role for syntactic bootstrapping. Developmental Psychology, 41, 529–540. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
[ p. 131 ]
Matsuo, A., Kita, S., Shinya, Y., Wood, G. C. & Naigles, L.
(2012) Japanese two-year-olds use morphosyntax to learn novel verb meanings. Journal of Child Language, 39(3), 637–663. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Merlo, P. & Stevenson, S.
(2001) Automatic verb classification based on statistical distributions of argument structure. Computational Linguistics, 27, 373–408. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Naigles, L., Gleitman, H., & Gleitman, L.
(1993) Children acquire word meaning components from syntactic evidence. In E. Dromi (Ed.), Language and development (pp. 104–140). Norwood, NJ: Ablex.Google Scholar
Naigles, L., & Hoff-Ginsberg, E.
(1995) Input to verb learning: Evidence for the plausibility of syntactic bootstrapping. Developmental Psychology, 31, 827–837. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Naigles, L., & Lehrer, A.
(2002) Language-general and language-specific influences on children’s acquisition of argument structure: A comparison of French and English. Journal of Child Language, 29, 545–566. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
O’Grady, W., Yamashita, Y., & Cho, S.
(2008) Object drop in Japanese and Korean. Language Acquisition, 15, 58–68. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Olguin, R., & Tomasello, M.
(1993) Twenty-five-month-old children do not have a grammatical category of verb. Cognitive Development, 8, 245–272. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Oshima-Takane, Y., Ariyama, J., Kobayashi, T., Katerelos, M., & Poulin-Dubois, D.
(2011) Early verb learning in 20-month-old Japanese-speaking children. Journal of Child Language, 38, 455–484. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Pérez-Leroux, A., Pirvulescu, M., & Roberge, Y.
(2008) Null objects in child language: Syntax and the lexicon. Lingua, 118, 370–398. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Pérez-Leroux, A. T., Pirvulescu, M., Roberge, Y., Tieu, L., & Thomas, D.
(2006) Variable input and object drop in child language. In C. Gurski & M. Radisic. Proceedings of the 2006 Canadian Linguistics Association Annual Conference. Retrieved from http://​westernlinguistics​.ca​/Publications​/CLA2006​/CLA​-ACL2006​.htm
Pirvulescu, M.
(2006) Theoretical implications of clitic omission in early French: Spontaneous vs. clicited production. Catalan Journal of Linguistics, 5, 221–236.Google Scholar
Prévost, P.
(2009) The acquisition of French: The development of inflectional morphology and syntax in L1 acquisition, bilingualism, and L2 acquisition. Amsterdam: John Benjamins. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Pye, C.
(1994) A cross-linguistic approach to the causative alternation. In Y. Levy (Ed.), Other children, other languages (pp. 243–264). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.Google Scholar
Rissman, L., Legendre, G. & Landau, B.
(2013) Abstract morphosyntax in two- and three-year-old children: Evidence from priming. Language Learning and Development, 9, 278–292. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Roberge, Y.
(1990) The syntactic recoverability of null arguments. Montreal: McGill-Queen’s University Press.Google Scholar
Roberts, I.
(2010) Agreement and head movement. Cambridge: MIT Press. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Rowland, C., Chang, F., Ambridge, B., Pine, J., & Lieven, E. V. M.
(2012) The development of abstract syntax: Evidence from structural priming and the lexical boost. Cognition, 125, 49–63. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Sigurðsson, H. A.
(2011) Conditions on argument drop. Linguistic Inquiry, 42(2), 267–304. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
[ p. 132 ]
Smith, J.
(2011) Everyday talk informs toddlers’ novel verb generalizations. First Language, 31(4), 404–424. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Sportiche, D.
(1995) Clitic constructions. In J. Rooryck & L. Zaring (Eds.), Phrase structure and the lexicon (pp. 213–276). Dordrecht: Kluwer.Google Scholar
Theakston, A., Lieven, E., Pine, J., & Rowland, C.
(2001) The role of performance limitations in the acquisition of verb-argument structures: An alternative account. Journal of Child Language, 28, 127–152. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Tomasello, M.
(2000) Do young children have adult syntactic competence? Cognition, 74, 209–253. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Zesiger, P., Chillier-Zesiger, L., Arabatzi, M., Baranzini, L., Cronel-Ohayon, S., Franck, J., Frauenfelder, U., Hamann, C., & Rizzi, L.
(2010) The acquisition of pronouns by French children: A parallel study of production and comprehension. Applied Psycholinguistics, 31, 571–603. Crossref[ p. 133 ]Google Scholar