References
Abu-Rabia, S.
(2012) The role of morphology and short vowelization in reading morphological complex words in Arabic : Evidence for the domination of the Morpheme/Root-Based Theory in reading Arabic. Creative Education, 03 (04), 486–494. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Ågren, M.
(2005) Développement de la morphologie du nombre en français langue étrangère à l’écrit: Étude transversale. Doctoral diss, Lund University.
(2016) Apprendre l’orthographe profonde du français langue étrangère. Revue Française de Linguistique Appliquée, XXI ( 2 ), 95–108. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Alderson, J. C.
(2005) Diagnosing foreign language proficiency: The interface between learning and assessment. A&C Black.Google Scholar
Angelelli, P., Marinelli, C. V., De Salvatore, M., & Burani, C.
(2017) Morpheme-based reading and spelling in Italian children with developmental dyslexia and dysorthography : Morpheme-based reading and spelling. Dyslexia, 23 (4), 387–405. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Aro, M., & Wimmer, H.
(2003) Learning to read: English in comparison to six more regular orthographies. Applied Psycholinguistics, 24 (4), 621–635. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Bane, M.
(2008) Quantifying and measuring morphological complexity. In C. B. Chang & H. J. Haynie (Eds.), Proceedings of the 26th west coast conference on formal linguistics (pp. 69–76). Cascadilla Proceedings Project.Google Scholar
Bassetti, B.
(2017) Orthography affects second language speech: Double letters and geminate production in English. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 43 (11), 1835–1842. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Bassetti, B., & Atkinson, N.
(2015) Effects of orthographic forms on pronunciation in experienced instructed second language learners. Applied Psycholinguistics, 36 (1), 67–91. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Bassetti, B., & Cook, V.
(2018) Second language writing systems. De Gruyter. [URL]
Bassetti, B., Masterson, J., Cerni, T., & Mairano, P.
(2021) Orthographic forms affect speech perception in a second language: Consonant and vowel length in L2 English. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 47 (12), 1583–1603. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Buetler, K. A., de León Rodríguez, D., Laganaro, M., Müri, R., Nyffeler, T., Spierer, L., & Annoni, J.-M.
(2015) Balanced bilinguals favor lexical processing in their opaque language and conversion system in their shallow language. Brain and Language, 150 1, 166–176. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Burani, C., Marcolini, S., & Stella, G.
(2002) How early does morpholexical reading develop in readers of a shallow orthography? Brain and Language, 81 (1–3), 568–586. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Catach, N.
(1992) L’orthographe française. Traité théorique et pratique. Nathan Université.Google Scholar
Chaussoy, L., Lambert, E., & Quémart, P.
(2022) Morphological processing in written word production is based on orthography rather than semantics. Acta Psychologica, 229 1, 103670. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Deacon, S. H., Wade-Woolley, L., & Kirby, J. R.
(2009) Flexibility in young second-language learners: Examining the language specificity of orthographic processing. Journal of Research in Reading, 32 (2), 215–229. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Ellis, N. C.
(2006) Selective attention and transfer phenomena in L2 acquisition: Contingency, cue competition, salience, interference, overshadowing, blocking, and perceptual learning. Applied Linguistics, 27 (2), 164–194. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Ellis, N. C., Natsume, M., Stavropoulou, K., Hoxhallari, L., Daal, V. H. P., Polyzoe, N., Tsipa, M.-L., & Petalas, M.
(2004) The effects of orthographic depth on learning to read alphabetic, syllabic, and logographic scripts. Reading Research Quarterly, 39 (4), 438–468. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Erdener, V. D., & Burnham, D. K.
(2005) The role of audiovisual speech and orthographic information in nonnative speech production. Language Learning, 55 (2), 191–228. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Escudero, P., Hayes-Harb, R., & Mitterer, H.
(2008) Novel second-language words and asymmetric lexical access. Journal of Phonetics, 36 (2), 345–360. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Fayol, M.
(2016) Comment apprendre l’orthographe ?. In M. Fournier (Ed), Éduquer et former: Connaissances et débats en éducation et formation (pp. 211–215). Éditions Sciences Humaines.Google Scholar
Fayol, M., & Got, C.
(1991) Automatisme et contrôle dans la production écrite : Les erreurs d’accord sujet verbe chez l’enfant et l’adulte. L’année Psychologique, 91 (2), 187–205. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Fayol, M., & Jaffré, J.-P.
(2008) Orthographier (1. ed). Presses Univ. de France.Google Scholar
Fayol, M., Largy, P., & Lemaire, P.
(1994) Cognitive overload and orthographic errors: When cognitive overload enhances subject–verb agreement errors. A study in French written language. The Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology Section A, 47 (2), 437–464. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Gandolfi, E., Usai, M. C., Traverso, L., & Viterbori, P.
(2022) Inhibitory control and verb inflection in Italian preschool children. Journal of Child Language, 1–17. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Georgiou, G. P.
(2021) How do speakers of a language with a transparent orthographic system perceive the L2 vowels of a language with an opaque orthographic system? An analysis through a battery of behavioral tests. Languages, 6 (3), 118. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Görgen, R., De Simone, E., Schulte-Körne, G., & Moll, K.
(2021) Predictors of reading and spelling skills in German: The role of morphological awareness. Journal of Research in Reading, 44 (1), 210–227. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Gunnarsson-Largy, C.
(2014) Utiliser l’erreur pour détecter les automatismes et l’expertise en production écrite en FLE. In C. Gunnarsson-Largy & E. Auriac-Slusarczyk (Eds), Ecriture et réécriture chez les élèves : un seul corpus, divers genres discursifs et méthodologies d’analyse (pp. 287–302). Academia Bruylant.Google Scholar
(2022) La production écrite en français : Focus sur le processus de formulation [Habilitation à diriger des recherches, Université Toulouse – Jean Jaurès]. [URL]
Gunnarsson-Largy, C., & Largy, P.
(2018) L’effet de l’émotion sur la production orthographique d’adultes FL1 et FL2. Approche Neuropsychologique des Apprentissages chez l’Enfant, 30 (30), 445–452.Google Scholar
Hamada, M., & Koda, K.
(2008) Influence of first language orthographic experience on second language decoding and word learning. Language Learning, 58 (1), 1–31. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Katz, L., & Frost, R.
(1992) The reading process is different for different orthographies: The Orthographic Depth Hypothesis. In R. Frost & L. Katz (Eds.), Advances in psychology (Vol. 941, p. 67–84). North-Holland. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Kilgarriff, A., Baisa, V., Bušta, J., Jakubíček, M., Kovář, V., Michelfeit, J., Rychlý, P., & Suchomel, V.
(2014) The Sketch Engine: Ten years on. Lexicography, 1 (1), 7–36. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Largy, P., & Fayol, M.
(2001) Oral cues improve subject-verb agreement in written French. International Journal of Psychology, 36 (2), 121–131. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Li, P., Bates, E., & Macwhinney, B.
(1993) Processing a language without inflections: A reaction time study of sentence interpretation in Chinese. Journal of Memory and Language, 32 (2), 169–192. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Mairano, P., Bassetti, B., Sokolović-Perović, M., & Cerni, T.
(2018) Effects of L1 orthography and L1 phonology on L2 English pronunciation: Revue Française de Linguistique Appliquée, Vol. XXIII(1), 45–57. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
McClung, N. A., & Pearson, P. D.
McDonald, J. L.
(2006) Beyond the critical period: Processing-based explanations for poor grammaticality judgment performance by late second language learners. Journal of Memory and Language, 55 (3), 381–401. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Nassaji, H.
(2014) The role and importance of lower-level processes in second language reading. Language Teaching, 47 (1), 1–37. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Ostrogonac, S., Pakoci, E., Sečujski, M., & Mišković, D.
(2019) Morphology-based vs unsupervised word clustering for training language models for Serbian. Acta Polytechnica Hungarica: Journal of Applied Sciences, 16 (2), 183–197.Google Scholar
Paulesu, E., McCrory, E., Fazio, F., Menoncello, L., Brunswick, N., Cappa, S. F., Cotelli, M., Cossu, G., Corte, F., Lorusso, M., Pesenti, S., Gallagher, A., Perani, D., Price, C., Frith, C. D., & Frith, U.
(2000) A cultural effect on brain function. Nature Neuroscience, 3 (1), 91–96. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Perfetti, C. A., Rieben, L., & Fayol, M.
(1997) Learning to spell: Research, theory, and practice across languages. Routledge. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Ranta, A.
(2008) How predictable is Finnish morphology? An experiment on lexicon construction. In J. Nivre, M. Dahllöf, & B. Megyesi (Eds.), Resourceful language technology: Festschrift in Honor of Anna Sågvall Hein (pp. 130–148). Acta Universitatis Upsaliensis.Google Scholar
Rau, A. K., Moll, K., Moeller, K., Huber, S., Snowling, M. J., & Landerl, K.
(2016) Same same, but different: Word and sentence reading in German and English. Scientific Studies of Reading, 20 (3), 203–219. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Rodríguez, E., & Reglero, L.
(2015) Heritage and L2 processing of person and number features: Evidence from Spanish subject-verb agreement. EuroAmerican Journal of Applied Linguistics and Languages, 2 (2), 11–30. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Schmalz, X., Marinus, E., Coltheart, M., & Castles, A.
(2015) Getting to the bottom of orthographic depth. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 22 (6), 1614–1629. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Schüppert, A., Heeringa, W., Golubovic, J., & Gooskens, C.
(2017) Write as you speak? A cross-linguistic investigation of orthographic transparency in 16 Germanic, Romance and Slavic languages. In M. Wieling, M. Kroon, G. van Noord, & G. Bouma (Eds.), From semantics to dialectometry: Festschrift in honour of John Nerbonne (pp. 303–313). College Publications.Google Scholar
Seymour, P. H. K., Aro, M., Erskine, J. M., & collaboration with COST Action A8 network
(2003) Foundation literacy acquisition in European orthographies. British Journal of Psychology, 94 (2), 143–174. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Shankweiler, D., Liberman, I. Y., & Michigan, U.
of (1989) Phonology and reading disability: Solving the reading puzzle. University of Michigan Press. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Shen, Y., & Del Tufo, S. N.
(2022) The influence of orthographic depth on multilinguals’ neural networks. Neuropsychologia, 164 1, 108095. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Shuster, V. P., & Miozzo, M.
(2018) The processing of inflected and derived words in writing. Cognitive Neuropsychology, 35 (7), 385–401. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Solier, C.
(2019) L’interface oral-écrit dans l’apprentissage d’une langue étrangère. Influence de l’input orthographique sur les représentations phonologiques : Le cas des apprenants marocains, Université de Toulouse. [URL]
Totereau, C., Barrouillet, P., & Fayol, M.
(1998) Overgeneralizations of number inflections in the learning of written French: The case of noun and verb. British Journal of Developmental Psychology, 16 (4), 447–464. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
van Daal, V. H. P., & Wass, M.
(2017) First- and second-language learnability explained by orthographic depth and orthographic learning: A “natural” Scandinavian experiment. Scientific Studies of Reading, 21 (1), 46–59. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Wobbrock, J. O., Findlater, L., Gergle, D., & Higgins, J. J.
(2011) The aligned rank transform for nonparametric factorial analyses using only Anova procedures. Proceedings of the SIGCHI Conference on Human Factors in Computing Systems, 143–146. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Ziegler, J. C., & Goswami, U.
(2005) Reading acquisition, developmental dyslexia, and skilled reading across languages: A psycholinguistic grain size theory. Psychological Bulletin, 131 (1), 3–29. DOI logoGoogle Scholar