References
Aksu-Koç, A
(1998) The role of input vs. universal predispositions in the emergence of tense-aspect morphology: evidence from Turkish. First Language 181, 255–280. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Aksu-Koç, A. & Ketrez, F.N
(2003) Early verbal morphology in Turkish: emergence of inflections. In W.U. Dressler, D. Bittner & M. Kilani-Schoch (eds.), Development of verb inflection in first language acquisition: a cross linguistic perspective (27–52). Berlin: Mouton de Gruyter. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Aikhenvald, A
(2004) Evidentiality. New York: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Bates, E. & Goodman, J.C
(1999) On the inseparability of grammar and the lexicon: evidence from acquisition. In M. Tomasello & E. Bates (eds.), Language development: the essential readings (134–162). Oxford: Blackwell Publishing.Google Scholar
Behrens, H
(2006) The input-output relationship in first language acquisition. Language and Cognitive Processes 211, 2–24. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Brodsky, P., Waterfall, H. & Edelman, S
(2007) Characterizing motherese: on the computational structure of child-directed language. In D.S. McNamara & J.G. Trafton (eds.), Proceedings of the 29th Meeting of the Cognitive Science Society (833–838). Austin, T.X.: Cognitive Science Society.Google Scholar
Bybee J., Perkins, R. & Pagliuca, W
(1994) The evolution of grammar: tense, aspect, and modality in the languages of the world. Chicago: The University of Chicago Press.Google Scholar
Choi S
(1995) The development of epistemic sentence-ending modal forms and functions in Korean children. In J. Bybee & S. Fleischman (eds.), Modality in grammar and discourse (165–204). Amsterdam: John Benjamins. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Choi, S
(2006) Acquisition of modality. In W. Frawley (ed.), The expression of modality (141–171). Berlin: Walter de Gruyter.Google Scholar
Choi, S. & Gopnik, A
(1995) Early acquisition of verbs in Korean: a cross-linguistic study. Journal of Child Language 221, 497–529. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Dabrowska, E. & Szczerbinski, M
(2006) Polish children’s productivity with case marking: the role of regularity, type frequency, and phonological diversity. Journal of Child Language 331, 559–597. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
de Haan, F
(2006) Typological approaches to modality. In W. Frawley (ed.), The expression of modality (27–69). Berlin: Walter de Gruyter.Google Scholar
Dressler, W.U
(ed.) (1997) Studies in pre- and protomorphology. Wien: Verlag der Österreichi-schen Akademie der Wissenschaften.Google Scholar
Dressler W.U. & Karpf, A
(1995) The theoretical relevance of pre- and protomorphology in language acquisition. In G. Booj & J. van Merle (eds.) Yearbook of Morphology 19941 (99–122). Dordrecht: Kluwer. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Dressler, W.U., Kilani-Schoch, M. & Klampfer, S
(2003) How does a child detect morphology? Evidence from production. In H.R. Baayen & R. Schreuder (eds.), Morphologial structure in languge processing (391–425). Berlin: Mouton de Gruyter. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Gentner, D
(1982) Why nouns are learned before verbs: linguistic relativity versus natural partioning. In S. Kuczaj (ed.), Language development, Vol. II: language, thought and culture (301–334). Hillsdale: Lawrence Erlbaum.Google Scholar
Givón, T
(2009) The ontogeny of complex verb phrases: how children learn to negotiate fact and desire. In T. Givón & M. Shibatani (eds.), Syntactic complexity: diachrony, acquisition, neuro-cognition, evolution (311–388). Amsterdam: John Benjamins. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Hickmann, M
(2003) Children’s discourse: person, space and time across languages. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Huttenlocher, J., Vasilyeva, M., Cymerman, E. & Levine, S
(2002) Language input and child syntax. Cognitive Psychology 451, 337–374. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Ingram, D. & Thompson, W
(1996) Early syntactic acquisition in German: evidence for the modal hypothesis. Language 721, 97–120. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Ketrez, F.N
(2003a) Variation in a Turkish mother’s style. In S. Özsoy, D. Akar, M. Nakipoğlu-Demiralp, E. Erguvanlı-Taylan & A. Aksu-Koç (eds.), Studies in Turkish linguistics (447–453). Istanbul: Boğaziçi University.Google Scholar
(2003b) Distributional properties of nouns and verbs in Turkish child-directed speech. USC Working Papers in Linguistics 11, 121–144.Google Scholar
Ketrez, F.N. & Aksu-Koç, A
(2009) Early nominal morphology in Turkish: emergence of case and number. In U. Stephany & M. Voeykova (eds.), Development of noun inflection in first language acquisition: a cross linguistic perspective (15–48). Berlin: Mouton de Gruyter. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Küntay, A. & Slobin, D.I
(1996) Listening to a Turkish mother: some puzzles for acquisition. In D.I. Slobin, J. Gerhardt, A. Kyratzis & J. Guo (eds.), Social interaction, social context and language: essays in honour of Susan Ervin-Tripp (265–286). Hillsdale, N.J.: Lawrence Erlbaum.Google Scholar
(2001) Discourse behavior of lexical categories in Turkish child-directed speech: nouns vs. verbs. In M. Almgren, A. Barreña, M. Ezeizabarrena, I. Idiazabal & B. MacWhinney (eds.), Research on child language acquisition: Proceedings for the 8th Conference of the International Association for the Study of Child Language (928–946). Somerville: Cascadilla Press.Google Scholar
Lee, C
(2009) The acquisition of modality. In C. Lee, G. Simpson, & Y. Kim (eds.), The handbook of Korean psycholinguistics (187–220). London: Cambridge University Press. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Lyons, J
(1977) Semantics. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
MacWhinney, B
(2000) The CHILDES project: tools for analyzing talk. Mahwah, N.J.: Lawrence Erlbaum.Google Scholar
MacWhinney, B., Bates, E. & Kliegl, R
(1984) Cue validity and sentence interpretation in English, German and Italian. Journal of Verbal Learning and Verbal Behavior 231, 127–150. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Maratsos, M
(1982) The child’s construction of grammatical categories. In E. Wanner & L.R. Gleitman (eds.), Language acquisition: the state of the art (240–266). Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Marchman, V. & Bates, E
(1994) Continuity in lexical and morphological development: a test of the critical mass hypothesis. Journal of Child Language 211, 339–366. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Naigles, L. & Hoff-Ginsberg, E
(1998) Why are some verbs learned before other verbs? Effects of input frequency and structure on children’s early verb use. Journal of Child Language 251, 95–120. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Palmer, F
(2001) Mood and modality (second edition). Cambridge: Cambridge University Press. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Pan, B.A., Rowe, M.L., Singer, J.D. & Snow, C.E
(2005) Maternal Correlates of growth in toddler vocabulary production in low-income families. Child Development 761, 762–782.Google Scholar
Papafragou, A
(2000) Modality: issues in the semantics-pragmatics interface. Amsterdam: Elsevier Science.Google Scholar
Portner, P
(2009) Modality. Oxford: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Rozendaal, M
(2008) The acquisition of reference: a crosslinguistic study. Utrecht: LOT.Google Scholar
Shepherd, S
(1982) From deontic to epistemic: an analysis of modals in the history of English, creoles, and language acquisition. In A. Ahlqvist (ed.), Papers from the 5th International Conference on Historical Linguistics (316–123). Amsterdam: John Benjamins. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Slobin, D.I
(1985) Crosslinguistic evidence for the language-making capacity. In D.I. Slobin (ed.), Crosslinguistic study of language acquisition (Vol. 21, 1157–1256). Hillsdale, N.J.: Lawrence Erlbaum.Google Scholar
(2001) Form-function relations: how do children find out what they are? In M. Bowerman & S.C. Levinson (eds.), Language acquisition and conceptual development (406–449). Cambridge: Cambridge University Press. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Stephany U
(1986) Modality. In P. Fletcher & M. Garman (eds.), Language acquisition (375–400). Cambridge: Cambridge University Press. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Stephany, U
(1993) Modality in first language acquisition: the state of the art. In N. Dittmar & A. Reich (eds.), Modality in language acquisition (133–144). New York: Walter de Gruyter.Google Scholar
Sofu, H. & Türkay, F
(2006) Input frequency effects in terms of noun/verb dominance. Çukurova Üniversitesi, Eğitim Fakültesi Dergisi 3/321, 229–241.Google Scholar
Tardif, T., Gelman, S.A. & Xu, F
(1999) Putting the “noun bias” in context: a comparison of English and Mandarin. Child Development 70(3), 620–635. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Terziyan, T., Taylan, E, & Aksu-Koç, A
in press). Acquisition of modality in Turkish. Proceedings of the International Conference on Turkish Linguistics , 2012. Harrasowitz Verlag. DOI logo
Tomasello, M
(2003) Constructing a language: a usage based theory of language acquisition. Cambridge, M.A.: Harvard University Press.Google Scholar
Türkay, F. & Kern, S
(2007) Türk ve Fransız anneler tarafından çocuğa yönlendirilmiş konuşmadaki farklılıklar: ad/eylem kullanımına karşılaştırmalı bir bakış açısı. XXI . Ulusal Dilbilim Kurultayı Bildirileri (373–376). Mersin, 10-11 Mayıs 2007. [Differences in the child directed speech of Turkish and French mothers: A contrastive perspective on noun/verb usage. Proceedings of the XXI. National Linguistics Conference , Mersin, May 10-11, 2007.]Google Scholar
Wells, G
(1979) Learning and using the auxiliary verb in English. In V. Lee (ed.), Language Development (250–270). London: Croom Helm.Google Scholar
Weizman, Z.O. & Snow, C.E
(2001) Lexical input as related to children’s vocabulary acquisition: effects of sophisticated exposure and support for meaning. Developmental Psychology 371, 265–279. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Wijnen, F.N.K
(1998) The temporal interpretation of Dutch children’s root infinitivals: the effect of eventivity. First language 181, 379–402. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Xanthos, A., Laaha, S., Gillis, S., Stephany, U., Aksu-Koç, A., Christofidou, A., Gagarina, N., Hrzica, G., Ketrez, F.N., Kilani-Schoch, M., Korecky-Kröll, K., Kovačević, M., Laalo, K., Palmović, M., Pfeiler, B., Voeikova, M.D. & Dressler, W.U
(2011) On the role of morphological richness in the early development of noun and verb inflection. First Language 31(4), 461–479. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Cited by

Cited by 6 other publications

Arslan, Seçkin, Roelien Bastiaanse & Claudia Felser
2015. Looking at the evidence in visual world: eye-movements reveal how bilingual and monolingual Turkish speakers process grammatical evidentiality. Frontiers in Psychology 6 DOI logo
Bassano, Dominique & Paul van Geert
2018. Chapter 10. New perspectives on input-output dynamics. In Sources of Variation in First Language Acquisition [Trends in Language Acquisition Research, 22],  pp. 201 ff. DOI logo
Coşkun Kunduz, Aylin & Silvina Montrul
2023. Input factors in the acquisition of evidentiality by Turkish heritage language children and adults in the United States. Language Acquisition  pp. 1 ff. DOI logo
Haznedar, Belma & F. Nihan Ketrez
2016. Introduction. In The Acquisition of Turkish in Childhood [Trends in Language Acquisition Research, 20],  pp. 1 ff. DOI logo
Karayayla, Tuğba
Ketrez, F. Nihan
2017. Chapter 7. Sibling influence on morphological development?. In Social Environment and Cognition in Language Development [Trends in Language Acquisition Research, 21],  pp. 99 ff. DOI logo

This list is based on CrossRef data as of 3 april 2024. Please note that it may not be complete. Sources presented here have been supplied by the respective publishers. Any errors therein should be reported to them.