References

References

Acredolo, L. P., & Goodwyn, S. W.
(1985) Symbolic gesturing in language development. Human Development 28(1), 40–49. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
(1988) Symbolic gesturing in normal infants. Child Development 591, 450–466. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Bates, E.
(1976) Language and context: The acquisition of pragmatics. New York, NY: Academic Press.Google Scholar
Bates, E., Benigni, L., Bretherton, I., Camaioni, L., & Volterra, V.
(1979) The emergence of symbols: Cognition and communication in infancy. New York, NY: Academic Press.Google Scholar
Benedicto, E., & Brentari, D.
(2004) Where did all the arguments go? Argument-changing properties of classifiers in ASL. Natural Language & Linguistic Theory 22(4), 743–810. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Boyatzis, C. J., & Watson, M. W.
(1993) Preschool children's symbolic representation of objects through gestures. Child Development 64(3), 729–735. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Brentari, D., DiRenzo, A., Keane, J., & Volterra, V.
(2015) Cognitive, Cultural, and Linguistic Sources of a Handshape Distinction Expressing Agentivity. Topics in Cognitive Science 71, 95–123. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Butcher, C., Mylander, C., & Goldin-Meadow, S.
(1991) Displaced communication in a self-styled gesture system: Pointing at the non-present. Cognitive Development 61, 315–342. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Calbris, G.
(2011) Elements of meaning in gesture (Vol. 51). Amsterdam: John Benjamins. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Capirci, O., Contaldo, A., Caselli, M. C., & Volterra, V.
Cartmill, E. A., Demir, O. E., & Goldin-Meadow, S.
(2011) Studying gesture. In E. Hoff (Ed.), The guide to research methods in child language (pp. 208–225). West Sussex, England: Wiley-Blackwell.Google Scholar
Cartmill, E. A., Novack, M., Loftus, W., Goldin-Meadow, S.
in preparation) Tracing the development of gesture: Does iconic gesture become more abstract over time?
Caselli, M. C., Rinaldi, P., Stefanini, S., & Volterra, V.
(2012) Early Action and Gesture “Vocabulary” and Its Relation With Word Comprehension and Production. Child Development 83(2), 526–542. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Emmorey, K., & Herzig, M.
(2003) Categorical versus gradient properties of classifier constructions in ASL. In K. Emmorey (Ed.), Perspectives on Classifier Constructions in Signed Languages (pp. 221–246). Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Feldman, H., Goldin-Meadow, S., & Gleitman, L.
(1978) Beyond Herodotus: The creation of language by linguistically deprived deaf children. In A. Lock (Ed.), Action, symbol, and gesture: The emergence of language (pp. 351–414). New York, NY: Academic Press.Google Scholar
Franklin, A., Giannakidou, A., & Goldin-Meadow, S.
(2011) Negation, questions, and structure building in a homesign system. Cognition 1181, 398–416. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Givón, T.
(1979) On understanding grammar. New York: Academic Press.Google Scholar
Goldin-Meadow, S.
(1979) Structure in a manual communication system developed without a conventional language model: Language without a helping hand. In H. Whitaker & H. A. Whitaker (Eds.), Studies in Neurolinguistics (Vol. 41). New York: Academic Press. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
(2003) The resilience of language: What gesture creation in deaf children can tell us about how all children learn language. New York: Psychology Press.Google Scholar
(2014) How gesture helps children learn language. In I. Arnon, M. Casillas, C. Kurumada & B. Estigarribia (Eds.) Language in Interaction. Studies in honor of Eve V. Clark, (pp. 157–171). Amsterdam: John Benjamins.Google Scholar
Goldin-Meadow, S., Butcher, C., Mylander, C., & Dodge, M.
(1994) Nouns and verbs in a self-styled gesture system: What’s in a name? Cognitive Psychology 271, 259–319. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Goldin-Meadow, S., Levine, S. C., Hedges, L. V., Huttenlocher, J., Raudenbush, S., & Small, S.
(2014) New evidence about language and cognitive development based on a longitudinal study: Hypotheses for intervention. American Psychologist 69(6), 588–599. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Goldin-Meadow, S., & Mylander, C.
(1984) Gestural communication in deaf children: The effects and non-effects of parental input on early language development. Monographs of the Society for Re- search in Child Development 491, 1–121. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
(1998) Spontaneous sign systems created by deaf children in two cultures. Nature 911, 279–281. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Goldin-Meadow, S., Mylander, C., & Butcher, C.
(1995) The resilience of combinatorial structure at the word level: Morphology in self-styled gesture systems. Cognition 56(3), 195–262. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Goldin-Meadow, S., Mylander, C., & Franklin, A.
(2007) How children make language out of gesture: Morphological structure in gesture systems developed by American and Chinese deaf children. Cognitive Psychology 55(2), 87–135. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Goodrich, W., & Hudson Kam, C. L.
(2009) Co‐speech gesture as input in verb learning. Developmental science 12(1), 81–87. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Hawkins, John A.
(1988) Explaining language universals. In J. A. Hawkins (Ed.), Explaining language universals (pp. 3–28). Cambridge, MA: Blackwell.Google Scholar
Hopper, P. J. & Thompson, S. A.
(1984) The iconicity of the universal categories ‘noun’ and ‘verbs’. In J. Haiman (Ed.), Iconicity in syntax (pp.151–183). Amsterdam: John Benjamins.Google Scholar
Hunsicker, D., & Goldin-Meadow, S.
Iverson, J. M., Capirci, O., & Caselli, M. C.
(1994) From communication to language in two modalities. Cognitive Development 9(1), 23–43. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Marentette, P., & Nicoladis, E.
(2011) Preschoolers’ interpretations of gesture: Label or action associate? Cognition 121(3), 386–399. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Marentette, P., Pettenati, P., Bello, A., & Volterra, V.
(2016) Gesture and Symbolic Representation in Italian and English‐Speaking Canadian 2‐Year‐Olds. Child development 87(3), 944–961. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
McNeill, D.
(1992) Hand and mind: What gestures reveal about thought. Chicago, IL: University of Chicago Press.Google Scholar
Meier, R. P., Mauk, C. E., Cheek, A., & Moreland, C. J.
(2008) The form of children’s early signs: iconic or motoric determinants?. Language learning and development 4(1), 63–98. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Mizuguchi, T., & Sugai, K.
(2002) Object-related knowledge and the production of gestures with imagined objects by preschool children. Perceptual and Motor Skills 94(1), 71–79. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Mitchell, R. W., & Clark, H.
(2015) Experimenter’s pantomimes influence children’s use of body part as object and imaginary object pantomimes: A replication. Journal of Cognition and Development 161, 703–718. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Namy, L. L.
(2008) Recognition of iconicity doesn't come for free. Developmental science 11(6), 841–846. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Namy, L. L., Campbell, A. L., & Tomasello, M.
(2004) The changing role of iconicity in non-verbal symbol learning: A U-shaped trajectory in the acquisition of arbitrary gestures. Journal of Cognition and Development 5(1), 37–57. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Nicoladis, E., Mayberry, R. I., & Genesee, F.
(1999) Gesture and early bilingual development. Developmental Psychology 35(2), 514. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Novack, M. A., Goldin-Meadow, S., & Woodward, A. L.
(2015) Learning from gesture: How early does it happen? Cognition 1421, 138–147. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Novack, M., Cartmill, E. A., Loftus, W. & Goldin-Meadow, S.
in preparation). Tracing the development of gesture: Does iconic gesture become more abstract over time?
Ortega, G., & Ozyurek, A.
(2016) Generalisable patterns of gesture distinguish semantic categories in communication without language. In 38th Annual Meeting of the Cognitive Science Society (CogSci 2016) (pp. 1182–1187). Cognitive Science Society.Google Scholar
Overton, W. F., & Jackson, J. P.
(1973) The representation of imagined objects in action sequences: A developmental study. Child Development 44(2), 309–314. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Özçalışkan, Ş., & Goldin-Meadow, S.
(2005) Gesture is at the cutting edge of early language development. Cognition 96(3), B101–B113. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Özçalışkan, S., & Goldin-Meadow, S.
(2009) When gesture – speech combinations do and do not index linguistic change. Language and Cognitive Processes 241, 190–217. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Özçalışkan, Ş., & Goldin-Meadow, S.
(2011) Is there an iconic gesture spurt at 26 months? In G. Stam, & M. Ishino (Eds.), Integrating gestures: The interdisciplinary nature of gesture (pp. 163–174). Amsterdam: John Benjamins. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Padden, C. A., Meir, I., Hwang, S. -O., Lepic, R., Seegers, S., & Sampson, T.
(2013) Patterned iconicity in sign language lexicons. Gesture 13(3), 287–308. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Pettenati, P., Stefanini, S., & Volterra, V.
(2010) Motoric characteristics of representational gestures produced by young children in a naming task. Journal of Child language 37(4), 887–911. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Rissman, L. & Goldin-Meadow, S.
in press) The development of causal structure without a language model. Language Learning and Development.
Rissman, L., & Goldin-Meadow, S.
(2017) The Development of Causal Structure without a Language Model. Language Learning and Development, 1–14. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Rowe, M. L., & Goldin-Meadow, S.
(2009) Early gesture selectively predicts later language learning. Developmental Science 121, 182–187. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Sapir, E.
(1921) Language: An introduction to the study of speech. New York: Harcourt Brace Jovanovich.Google Scholar
Schachter, P.
(1985) Parts-of-speech systems. In T. Shopen (Ed.). Language typology and syntactic description: Clause Structure Vol. 1 (pp. 3–61). Cambridge, MA: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Stanfield, C., Williamson, R., & Özçalişkan, Ş. E. Y. D. A.
(2014) How early do children understand gesture – speech combinations with iconic gestures? Journal of Child Language 41(2), 462–71. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Supalla, T., & Newport, E.
(1978) How many seats in a chair? The derivation of nouns and verbs in American Sign Language. In P. Siple (Ed.), Understanding language through sign language research. New York: Academic Press.Google Scholar
Tolar, T. D., Lederberg, A. R., Gokhale, S., & Tomasello, M.
(2008) The development of the ability to recognize the meaning of iconic signs. Journal of Deaf Studies and Deaf Education,13(2), 225–240. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Cited by

Cited by 10 other publications

Blondel, Marion, Dominique Boutet, Pauline Beaupoil-Hourdel & Aliyah Morgenstern
2017. La négation chez les enfants signeurs et non signeurs. Language, Interaction and Acquisition 8:1  pp. 141 ff. DOI logo
Bohn, Manuel, Josep Call & Michael Tomasello
2019. Natural reference: A phylo‐ and ontogenetic perspective on the comprehension of iconic gestures and vocalizations. Developmental Science 22:2 DOI logo
Boutet, Dominique, Marion Blondel, Pauline Beaupoil-Hourdel & Aliyah Morgenstern
2021. A multimodal and kinesiological approach to the development of negation in signing and non-signing children. Languages and Modalities 1  pp. 31 ff. DOI logo
Debras, Camille & Pauline Beaupoil-Hourdel
2019. Gestualité et construction des chaînes de référence dans un corpus d’interactions tandem. Cahiers de praxématique :72 DOI logo
Holler, Judith
2022. Visual bodily signals as core devices for coordinating minds in interaction. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences 377:1859 DOI logo
Karadöller, Dilay Z., Beyza Sümer & Aslı Özyürek
2021. Effects and Non-Effects of Late Language Exposure on Spatial Language Development: Evidence from Deaf Adults and Children. Language Learning and Development 17:1  pp. 1 ff. DOI logo
KERN, Friederike, Ulrich BODEN, Anne NEMETH, Sofia KOUTALIDIS, Olga ABRAMOV, Stefan KOPP & Katharina J. ROHLFING
2024. Preschool children’s discourse competence in different genres and how it relates to iconic gestures. Journal of Child Language  pp. 1 ff. DOI logo
Koulidobrova, Elena & Deborah Chen Pichler
2021. Cripping the “Delay”: Multilingualism-Related Consequences of Re-Labeling Language Deprivation Systems. Frontiers in Communication 6 DOI logo
Novack, Miriam A. & Susan Goldin-Meadow
2023. Harnessing gesture to understand and support healthy development. In Encyclopedia of Child and Adolescent Health,  pp. 40 ff. DOI logo
Novack, Miriam A. & Sandra Waxman
2020. Becoming human: human infants link language and cognition, but what about the other great apes?. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences 375:1789  pp. 20180408 ff. DOI logo

This list is based on CrossRef data as of 3 march 2024. Please note that it may not be complete. Sources presented here have been supplied by the respective publishers. Any errors therein should be reported to them.