Article published in:
Methodology in Linguistic Landscape Research
Edited by Robert Blackwood
[Linguistic Landscape 3:3] 2017
► pp. 226245
References

References

Backhaus, P.
(2007) Linguistic landscapes: A comparative study of urban multilingualism in Tokyo. Bristol, New York, Toronto: Multilingual Matters.Google Scholar
[ p. 243 ]
Ben-Rafael, E., Shohamy, E., Hasan Amara, M., & Trumper-Hecht, N.
(2006) Linguistic landscape as symbolic construction of the public space: The case of Israel. International Journal of Multilingualism, 3(1), 7–30. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Bogatto, F., & Hélot, C.
(2010) Linguistic landscape and language diversity in Strasbourg: The ‘Quartier Gare’. In E. Shohamy, E. Ben-Rafael & M. Barni (Eds.), Linguistic landscape in the City (pp. 275–291). Bristol, New York, Toronto: Multilingual Matters.Google Scholar
Castelhano, M. S., & Henderson, J. M.
(2008) The influence of color on the perception of scene gist. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 34(3), 660.Google Scholar
Fei-Fei, L., Iyer, A., Koch, C., & Perona, P.
(2007) What do we perceive in a glance of a real-world scene?. Journal of vision, 7(1), 10–10. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Findlay, J. M., & Walker, R.
(1999) A model of saccade generation based on parallel processing and competitive inhibition. Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 22(04), 661–674. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Gorter, D.
(2013) Linguistic landscapes in a multilingual world. Annual Review of Applied Linguistics, 33, 190–212. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
(2016, April). Developments in Linguistic Landscape methodologies. Invited talk presented at the 37th International LAUD Symposium. University of Koblenz-Landau, Germany.
Henderson, J. M.
(2003) Human gaze control during real-world scene perception. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 7(11), 498–504. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Henderson, J. M., & Ferreira, F.
(2004) Scene perception for psycholinguists. In J. M. Henderson and F. Ferreira (Eds.), The interface of language, vision, and action: Eye movements and the visual world. New York: Psychology Press.Google Scholar
(2013) The Interface of Language, Vision, and Action: Eye movements and the visual world. Psychology Press.Google Scholar
Higgins, E., Leinenger, M., & Rayner, K.
(2014) Eye movements when viewing advertisements. Frontiers in Psychology, 5. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Itti, L., & Koch, C.
(2000) A saliency-based search mechanism for overt and covert shifts of visual attention. Vision Research, 40(10), 1489–1506. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Lamarre, P.
(2014) Bilingual winks and bilingual wordplay in Montreal’s linguistic landscape. International Journal of the Sociology of Language, 2014(228), 131–151. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Landry, R., & Bourhis, R.
(1997) Linguistic landscape and ethnolinguistic vitality. Journal of Language and Social Psychology, 16(1), 23–49. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Leimgruber, J. R. E., Vingron, N., & Titone, D.
submitted. What do People Notice from Real-World Linguistic Landscapes? A Review of the Literature and Recommendations for Future Research. Questioning Boundaries, Opening Spaces: Reterritorialising Linguistic Landscapes.
Leimgruber, Jakob R. E.
(2017) Global multilingualism, local bilingualism, official monolingualism: The linguistic landscape of a downtown Montreal street. International Journal of Bilingual Education and Bilingualism, 1–16Google Scholar
Libben, M. R., & Titone, D. A.
(2009) Bilingual lexical access in context: evidence from eye movements during reading. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 35(2), 381.Google Scholar
Luna, D., & Peracchio, L. A.
(2001) Moderators of Language Effects in Advertising to Bilinguals: A Psycholinguistic Approach. Journal Of Consumer Research, 28(2), 284–295. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
[ p. 244 ]
McCay-Peet, L., Lalmas, M., & Navalpakkam, V.
(2012, May). On saliency, affect and focused attention. In Proceedings of the SIGCHI Conference on Human Factors in Computing Systems (pp. 541–550). ACM.
Pivneva, I., Mercier, J., & Titone, D.
(2014) Executive control modulates cross-language lexical activation during L2 reading: Evidence from eye movements. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 40(3), 787.Google Scholar
Rayner, K.
(1998) Eye movements in reading and information processing: 20 years of research. Psychological Bulletin, 124(3), 372. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
(2009) Eye movements and attention in reading, scene perception, and visual search. The Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 62(8), 1457–1506. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Rayner, K., Rotello, C. M., Stewart, A. J., Keir, J., & Duffy, S. A.
(2001) Integrating text and pictorial information: eye movements when looking at print advertisements. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Applied, 7(3), 219.Google Scholar
Robinson, P.
(1995) Attention, memory, and the “noticing” hypothesis. Language Learning, 45(2), 283–331. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Roth, S. P., Tuch, A. N., Mekler, E. D., Bargas-Avila, J. A., & Opwis, K.
(2013) Location matters, especially for non-salient features – An eye-tracking study on the effects of web object placement on different types of websites. International Journal of Human-Computer Studies, 71(3), 228–235. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Seifi, P.
(2015) Eye Movements and Linguistic Landscape. Unpublished master’s thesis, University of Groningen, The Netherlands.Google Scholar
Shohamy, E. G., Rafael, E. B., & Barni, M.
(Eds.) (2010) Linguistic Landscape in the City. Bristol, New York, Toronto: Multilingual Matters.Google Scholar
Titone, D., Pivneva, I., Sheikh, N. A., Webb, N., & Whitford, V. M.
(2015) Doubling Down on Multifactorial Approaches to the Study of Bilingualism & Executive Control. Bilingualism: Language and Cognition, 18(01), 43–44.Google Scholar
Tufi, S., & Blackwood, R.
(2010) Trademarks in the linguistic landscape: methodological and theoretical challenges in qualifying brand names in the public space. International Journal of Multilingualism, 7(3), 197–210. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
, M. L. H., & Henderson, J. M.
(2010) The time course of initial scene processing for eye movement guidance in natural scene search. Journal of Vision, 10(3), 14–14. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Whitford, V., & Titone, D.
(2012) Second-language experience modulates first-and second-language word frequency effects: Evidence from eye movement measures of natural paragraph reading. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 19(1), 73–80. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
(2014) The effects of reading comprehension and launch site on frequency – predictability interactions during paragraph reading. The Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 67(6), 1151–1165. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
(2015) Second-language experience modulates eye movements during first-and second-language sentence reading: evidence from a gaze-contingent moving window paradigm. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 41(4), 1118.Google Scholar
[ p. 245 ]
Whitford, V., Pivneva, I., & Titone, D.
(2016) Eye movement methods to investigate bilingual reading. In R. Heredia, J. Altarriba & A. Cieslicka (Eds.), Methods in Bilingual Reading Comprehension Research (pp. 183–211). Springer New York. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Cited by

Cited by 5 other publications

Amos, H. William
2020. English in French Commercial Advertising: Simultaneity, bivalency, and language boundaries. Journal of Sociolinguistics 24:1  pp. 55 ff. Crossref logo
Leimgruber, Jakob R. E.
2020. Global multilingualism, local bilingualism, official monolingualism: the linguistic landscape of Montreal's St. Catherine Street. International Journal of Bilingual Education and Bilingualism 23:6  pp. 708 ff. Crossref logo
Leimgruber, Jakob R. E. & Víctor Fernández-Mallat
2021. Language attitudes and identity building in the linguistic landscape of Montreal. Open Linguistics 7:1  pp. 406 ff. Crossref logo
Martínez Ibarra, Francisco
2021.  In Linguistic Landscape in the Spanish-speaking World [Issues in Hispanic and Lusophone Linguistics, 35],  pp. 293 ff. Crossref logo
Tiv, Mehrgol, Vincent Rouillard, Naomi Vingron, Sabrina Wiebe & Debra Titone
2019. Global Second Language Proficiency Predicts Self-Perceptions of General Sarcasm Use Among Bilingual Adults. Journal of Language and Social Psychology 38:4  pp. 459 ff. Crossref logo

This list is based on CrossRef data as of 03 november 2021. Please note that it may not be complete. Sources presented here have been supplied by the respective publishers. Any errors therein should be reported to them.