Does repeated pre-task planning have an impact on form-focused LREs?
Evidence from EFL children
Pre-task planning has been shown to improve fluency and complexity in adult learners’ speech in monologic tasks.
Previous work considered pre-task planning in interactive tasks with young children in an ESL setting and reported that, in
general, it had an impact on the amount of talk. However, no study so far has considered the impact of planning on the production
of language-related episodes (LREs) by young children. This study examined the relationship between planning time and the accurate
production of three target form-focused LREs (FFLREs) by thirty-three dyads of 11–12-year-old EFL learners who took part in a
longitudinal experiment in which they narrated picture-prompted stories four times under one of three conditions: unguided
planning (12 dyads), guided planning (12 dyads), or no-planning (9 dyads). Results showed that no significant improvement in
target feature production was observed across weeks or conditions. Regarding accuracy, a notable difference emerged between the
pre-test and delayed post-test in the unguided planning condition. Regarding between-group comparisons, although the guided
planning group initially demonstrated higher accuracy than their counterparts, this distinction was not maintained. Thus, planning
had limited benefits when considering FFLREs. Methodological and pedagogical implications will be discussed.
Article outline
- 1.Introduction
- 2.Literature review
- 2.1Pre-task planning and task repetition
- 2.2Research questions
- 3.Methods
- 3.1Participants
- 3.2Target features
- 3.3Data collection
- 3.4Data analysis
- 4.Results
- 5.Discussion
- 6.Conclusion and pedagogical implications
- Acknowledgements
- Note
-
References
References (60)
References
Azkarai, A., & García Mayo, M. P. (2017). Task
repetition effects on L1 use in EFL child task-based interaction. Language Teaching
Research, 21(4), 480–495.
Basturkmen, H., & Fu, M. (2021). Corrective
feedback and the development of second language grammar. In H. Nassaji & E. Kartchava (Eds.), The
Cambridge handbook of corrective feedback in second language
learning (pp. 367–386). Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.
Bryfonski, L., & McKay, T. H. (2019). TBLT
implementation and evaluation: A meta-analysis. Language Teaching
Research, 23(5), 603–632.
Cambridge Young Learners English
Assessment. (2018). Cambridge English A2
Flyers. Volume 11. Cambridge English language assessment. Retrieved March 28, 2020, from [URL]
Calzada, A., & García Mayo, M. P. (2021). Child
learners’ reflections about EFL grammar in a collaborative writing task: When form is not at odds with
communication. Language
Awareness, 20(1), 1–16.
Collins, L., & Muñoz, C. (2016). The
foreign language classroom: Current perspectives and future considerations. The Modern Language
Journal, 1001, 133–147.
Council of Europe. (2018). Common
European Framework of Reference for Languages: Learning, Teaching, Assessment. Companion volume
with new descriptors. Retrieved March
28, 2020 from [URL]
Ellis, R. (2019). Explicit
versus implicit oral corrective feedback. In H. Nassaji & E. Kartchava (Eds.), The
Cambridge handbook on corrective
feedback (pp. 341–364). Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.
Ellis, R., & Yuan, F. (2004). The
effects of planning on fluency, complexity, and accuracy in second language narrative
writing. Studies in Second Language
Acquisition, 26(1), 59–84.
Enever, J. (2018). Politics
and policy in global primary English. Oxford: Oxford University Press.
García Mayo, M. P. (2021). Task
interaction in task-supported EFL/CLIL contexts. In M. J. Ahmadian & M. H. Long (Eds.), The
Cambridge handbook of task-based language
teaching (pp. 397–415). Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.
García Mayo, M. P., & Imaz Aguirre, A. (2016). Task
repetition and its impact on EFL children’s negotiation of meaning strategies and pair dynamics: An exploratory
study. The Language Learning
Journal, 44(4), 451–466.
García Mayo, M. P., & Villarreal, I. (2011). The
development of suppletive and affixal tense and agreement morphemes in the L3 English of Basque-Spanish bilinguals. Second Language
Research, 27(1), 129–149.
Gass, S. M., & Mackey, A. (2007). Input,
interaction, and output in second language acquisition. In B. VanPatten & J. Williams (Eds.), Theories
in second language
acquisition (pp. 175–200). London: LEA.
Genessee, F. (Ed.). (1994). Educating
second language children: The whole child, the whole curriculum, the whole
communitiy. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.
Guara-Tavarés, M. (2016). Learners’
processes during pre-task planning and working memory capacity. Ilha do
Desterro, 691, 79–94.
Hidalgo, M. A., & García Mayo, M. P. (2021). The
influence of task repetition type on young EFL learners’ attention to form. Language Teaching
Research, 25(4), 565–586.
Kang, E. Y., Sok, S., & Han, Z. (2019). Thirty-five
years of ISLA on form-focused instruction: a meta-analysis. Language Teaching
Research, 23(4), 428–453.
Kasprowicz, R., & Marsden, E. (2018). Towards
ecological validity in research into input-based practice: Form spotting can be as beneficial as form-meaning
practice. Applied
Linguistics, 39(6), 886–911.
Kellerman, E. (1985). “If
at first you do succeed…” In S. Gass & C. Madden (Eds.), Input
in second language
acquisition, (pp. 345–353). Rowley, MA: Newbury House.
Khoram, A. (2019). The
impact of task type and pre-task planning condition on the accuracy of intermediate EFL learners’ oral
performance. Cogent
Education 6(1).
Kim, Y. (2013). Effects
of pretask modeling on attention to form and question development. TESOL
Quarterly, 47(1), 8–35.
Kim, Y., Kang, S., Yun, H., Kim, B., & Choi, B. (2020). The
role of task repetition in a Korean as a foreign language classroom: Writing quality, attention to form, and learning of
Korean grammar. Foreign Language
Annals, 53(4), 827–849.
Leeser, M. J. (2004). Learner
proficiency and focus on form during collaborative dialogue. Language Teaching
Research, 8(1), 55–81.
Levelt, W. (1989). Speaking:
From intention to articulation: MA: MIT Press.
Li, L., Chen, J., & Sun, L. (2015). The
effects of different lengths of pretask planning time on L2 learners’ oral test
performance. TESOL
Quarterly, 49(1), 38–66.
Lightbown, P. M. (1985). Great
expectations: Second-language acquisition research and classroom teaching.” Applied
Linguistics, 6(2), 173–189.
Markee, N., & Kunitz, S. (2013). Doing
planning and task performance in second language acquisition: An ethnomethodological
respecification. Language
Learning, 63(4), 629–664.
MacWhinney, B. (2000). The
CHILDES project: Tools for analyzing talk: Transcription format and programs. (3rd
ed.). Lawrence Erlbaum.
McLaughlin, B. (1990). “Conscious”
versus “Unconscious” learning. TESOL
Quarterly, 24(4), 617–634.
Mochizuki, N., & Ortega, L. (2008). Balancing
communication and grammar in beginning-level foreign language classrooms: A study of guided planning and
relativization. Language Teaching
Research, 121, 11–37.
Nassaji, H. (2017). Grammar
acquisition. In S. Loewen & M. Sato (Eds.), The
handbook of instructed second language
acquisition, (pp. 205–223). New York: Routledge.
Oliver, R., Nguyen, B., & Sato, M. (2017). Child
instructed SLA. In S. Loewen & M. Sato (Eds.), The
handbook of instructed second language
acquisition, (pp. 468–487). New York: Routledge.
Ortega, L. (1999). Planning
and focus on form in L2 oral performance. Studies in Second Language
Acquisition, 21(1), 109–148.
Park, S. (2010). The
influence of pretask instructions and pretask planning on focus on form during Korean EFL task-based
interaction. Language Teaching
Research, 14(1), 9–26.
Philp, J., Oliver, R., & Mackey, A. (2006). The
impact of planning time on children’s task-based
interactions. System, 34(4), 547–565.
Pica, T. (2002). Subject-matter
content: How does it assist the interactional and linguistic needs of classroom language
learners? The Modern Language
Journal, 86(1), 1–19.
Pinter, A. (2011). Children
learning second languages. Basingstoke: Palgrave Macmillan.
Qin, J. (2019). Effects
of repeated practice in pre-task planning on the acquisition of English personal pronouns by Chinese EFL
learners. System, 811, 100–109.
Robinson, P. (2001). Task
complexity, task difficulty, and task production: Exploring interactions in a componential
framework. Applied
Linguistics, 22(1), 27–57.
Romanova, N. (2010). Planning,
recasts, and learning of L2 morphology. Canadian Modern Language
Review, 66(6), 843–875.
Sato, M., & Dussuel-Lam, C. (2021). Metacognitive
instruction with young learners: A case of willingness to communicate, L2 use, and metacognition of oral
communication. Language Teaching
Research, 251, 899–921.
Sato, M., & Oyanedel, J. C. (2019). ‘I
think that is a better way to teach but ..’ EFL teachers’ conflicting beliefs about grammar
teaching. System, 841, 110–122.
Savignon, S. J. (1991). Communicative
language teaching: State of the art. TESOL
Quartely, 25(2), 261–278.
Skehan, P. (1998). A
cognitive approach to language learning. Oxford: Oxford University Press.
Skehan, P. (2015). Foreign
language aptitude and its relationship with grammar: A critical overview. Applied
Linguistics, 36(3), 367–384.
Spada, N., & Tomita, Y. (2010). Interactions
between type of instruction and type of language feature: A meta-analysis. Language
Learning, 60(2), 263–308.
Stansfield, Ch. W., Reed, D. J., & Velasco, A. M. (2005). The
Modern language aptitude test- elementary: Spanish version (MLAT-ES). Rockville, MD: Language Learning and Testing Foundation.
Swain, M., & Lapkin, S. (1998). Interaction
and second language learning: Two adolescent French immersion students working together. The
Modern Language
Journal, 82(3), 320–337.
VanPatten, B. (1990). Attention
to form and content in the input. An experiment in consciousness. Studies in Second Language
Acquisition, 12(3), 287–312.
Cited by (1)
Cited by one other publication
Guerrero-Gomez, Janire, María Orcasitas-Vicandi & Marta Kopinska
2024.
Enhancing young EFL learners’ written skills: the role of repeated pre-task planning.
International Review of Applied Linguistics in Language Teaching
This list is based on CrossRef data as of 19 september 2024. Please note that it may not be complete. Sources presented here have been supplied by the respective publishers.
Any errors therein should be reported to them.