References
Baayen, R. H., Davidson, D. J., & Bates, D. M.
(2008) Mixed-effects modeling with crossed random effects for subjects and items. Journal of Memory and Language, 591, 390–412. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Baayen, R. H., & Milin, P.
(2010) Analyzing reaction times. International Journal of Psychological Research, 3(2), 12–28. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Baayen, R. H., Piepenbrock, R., & van Rijn, H.
(1993) The CELEX lexical database (on CD-ROM). Philadelphia, PA: Linguistic Data Consortium, University of Pennsylvania.Google Scholar
Baayen, R. H., & Smolka, E.
(2019) Modeling morphological priming without morphemes. Manuscript submitted for publication. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Bates, D.
(2005) Fitting linear mixed models in R. R news, 5(1), 27–30.Google Scholar
Bodner, G. E., & Masson, M. E. J.
(2003) Beyond spreading activation: An influence of relatedness proportion on masked semantic priming. Psychonomic Bulletin and Review, 101, 645–652. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Boersma, P., & Weenink, D.
(2009) Praat: Doing phonetics by computer. Amsterdam. Retrieved from [URL]
Brunel, N., & Lavigne, F.
(2009) Semantic priming in a cortical network model. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 21(12), 2300–2319. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Collins, A. M., & Loftus, E. F.
(1975) A Spreading-Activation Theory of Semantic Processing. Psychological Review, 82(6), 407–428. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
De Grauwe, S., Lemhöfer, K., & Schriefers, H.
(2019) Processing derived verbs: the role of motor-relatedness and type of morphological priming. Language, Cognition and Neuroscience, 34(8), 973–990. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Dell, G. S.
(1986) A spreading-activation theory of retrieval in sentence production. Psychological Review, 931, 283–321. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Diependaele, K., Sandra, D., & Grainger, J.
(2005) Masked cross-modal morphological priming: Unravelling morpho-orthographic and morpho-semantic influences in early word recognition. Language and Cognitive Processes, 20(1–2), 75–114. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
(2009) Semantic transparency and masked morphological priming: the case of prefixed words. Memory and Cognition, 37(6), 895–908. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Dudenredaktion
(2009) Die deutsche Rechtschreibung. Das umfassende Standardwerk auf der Grundlage der neuen amtlichen Regeln (25th ed). Mannheim: Dudenverlag.Google Scholar
Eisenberg, P.
(2004) Grundriß der deutschen Grammatik: Das Wort (2nd ed. Vol. 11). Stuttgart-Weimar: J. B. Metzler. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Feldman, L. B., Barac-Cikoja, D., & Kostić, A.
(2002) Semantic aspects of morphological processing: Transparency effects in Serbian. Memory and Cognition, 301, 629–636. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Feldman, L. B., & Larabee, J.
(2001) Morphological facilitation following prefixed but not suffixed primes: Lexical architecture or modality-specific processes? Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 27(3), 680–691.Google Scholar
Feldman, L. B., & Soltano, E. G.
(1999) Morphological priming: The role of prime duration, semantic transparency, and affix position. Brain and Language, 681, 33–39. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Fickel, J. & Smolka, E.
(2014) The processing of German stems in zero derivation, Umlaut, and Ablaut. International Conference on the Cross-Linguistic Comparison of Indo-Germanic and Semitic Languages (CoGS), Konstanz, Germany.
Fleischer, W., & Barz, I.
(1992) Wortbildung der deutschen Gegenwartssprache. Tübingen: Max Niemeyer Verlag.Google Scholar
Gonnerman, L. M., Seidenberg, M. S., & Andersen, E. S.
(2007) Graded semantic and phonological similarity effects in priming: Evidence for a distributed connectionist approach to morphology. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 136(2), 323–345. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Günther, F., Smolka, E., & Marelli, M.
(2019) ‘Understanding’ differs between English and German: Capturing systematic language differences of complex words. Cortex, 1161, 158–175. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Ji, H., Gagné, C. L., & Spalding, T. L.
(2011) Benefits and costs of lexical decomposition and semantic integration during the processing of transparent and opaque English compounds. Journal of Memory and Language, 65(4), 406–430. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Longtin, C., Segui, J., & Hallé, P.
(2003) Morphological priming without morphological relationship. Language and Cognitive Processes, 181, 313–334. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Marslen-Wilson, W., Tyler, L. K., Waksler, R., & Older, L.
(1994) Morphology and meaning in the English mental lexicon. Psychological Review, 101(1), 3–33. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Milin, P., Smolka, E., & Feldman, L. B.
(2017) Models of Lexical Access and Morphological Processing. In E. M. Fernandéz & H. S. Cairns (Eds.), The Handbook of Psycholinguistics (pp. 240–268). Malden/Oxford: Wiley-Blackwell. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Pastizzo, M. J., & Feldman, L. B.
(2002) Does prime modality influence morphological processing? Brain and Language, 811, 28–41. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Rastle, K., Davis, M. H., Marslen-Wilson, W., & Tyler, L. K.
(2000) Morphological and semantic effects in visual word recognition: A time-course study. Language and Cognitive Processes, 15(4–5), 507–537. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Rueckl, J. G., & Galantucci, B.
(2005) The locus and time course of long-term morphological priming. Language and Cognitive Processes, 201, 115–138. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Smolka, E., Gondan, M., & Rösler, F.
(2015) Take a stand on understanding: Electrophysiological evidence for stem access in German complex verbs. Frontiers in Human Neuroscience, 9(62). DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Smolka, E., Komlósi, S., & Rösler, F.
(2009) When semantics means less than morphology: Processing of German prefixed verbs. Language and Cognitive Processes, 24(3), 337–375. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Smolka, E., & Libben, G.
(2017) ‘Can you wash off the hogwash?’ – semantic transparency of first and second constituents in the processing of German compounds. Language, Cognition, and Neuroscience, 32(4), 514–531. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Smolka, E., Libben, G., & Dressler, W. U.
(2019) When Morphological Structure Overrides Meaning: Evidence from German Prefix and Particle Verbs. Language, Cognition, and Neuroscience, 34(5), 599–614. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Smolka, E., & Eulitz, C.
(2018) Psycholinguistic measures for German verb pairs: Semantic trans-parency, semantic relatedness, verb family size, and age of reading acquisition. Behavior Research Methods, 50(4), 1540–1562. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Smolka, E., Preller, K., & Eulitz, C.
(2014) ‘Verstehen’ (‘understand’) primes ‘stehen’ (‘stand’): Morphological Structure Overrides Semantic Compositionality in the Lexical Representation of German Complex Verbs. Journal of Memory and Language, 721, 16–36. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Taft, M., & Forster, K. I.
(1975) Lexical storage and retrieval of prefixed words. Journal of Verbal Learning and Verbal Behavior, 141, 638–647. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Taft, M., & Nguyen-Hoan, M.
(2010) A sticky stick? The locus of morphological representation in the lexicon. Language and Cognitive Processes, 25(2), 277–296. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Xu, J., & Taft, M.
(2015) The effects of semantic transparency and base frequency on the recognition of English complex words. Journal of Experimental Psychology, 41(3), 904–910.Google Scholar
Zwitserlood, P., Bolwiender, A., & Drews, E.
(2005) Priming morphologically complex verbs by sentence contexts: Effects of semantic transparency and ambiguity. Language and Cognitive Processes, 201, 395–415. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Zwitserlood, P., Drews, E., Bolwiender, A., & Neuwinger, E.
(1996) Kann man Geschenke umbringen? Assoziative Bahnungsexperimente zur Bedeutungsheterogenität von Verben. In C. Habel & S. Kanngießer (Eds.), Perspektiven der kognitiven Linguistik: Modelle und Methoden (pp. 211–232). DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Cited by

Cited by 1 other publications

Creemers, Ava & David Embick
2021. Retrieving stem meanings in opaque words during auditory lexical processing. Language, Cognition and Neuroscience 36:9  pp. 1107 ff. DOI logo

This list is based on CrossRef data as of 10 april 2022. Please note that it may not be complete. Sources presented here have been supplied by the respective publishers. Any errors therein should be reported to them.