Article published In:
The Mental Lexicon
Vol. 18:3 (2023) ► pp.366400
References (61)
References
Abney, S. P. (1991). Parsing by chunks. In R. C. Berwick (Ed.), Principle-based parsing (pp. 257–278). Springer. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Attardi, G. (2006). Experiments with a multilanguage non-projective dependency parser. In The 10th International Conference on Computational Natural Language Learning (CoNLL-X2006) (pp. 166–170). New York.Google Scholar
Baddeley, A. (2007). Working memory, thought, and action (Vol. 451). OuP Oxford. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Bates, D., Maechler, M., Bolker, B., Walker, S., Christensen, R. H. B., Singmann, H., … others (2009). Package ‘lme4’. r-pro ject.org.Google Scholar
Brainard, D. H. (1997). The psychophysics toolbox. Spatial vision, 10 (4), 433–436. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Breen, M. (2014). Empirical investigations of the role of implicit prosody in sentence processing. Language and Linguistics Compass, 8 (2), 37–50. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Breen, M., Kaswer, L., Van Dyke, J. A., Krivokapić, J., & Landi, N. (2016). Imitated prosodic fluency predicts reading comprehension ability in good and poor high school readers. Frontiers in psychology, 7 1, 1026. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Brysbaert, M., & Stevens, M. (2018). Power analysis and effect size in mixed effects models: A tutorial. Journal of cognition, 1 (1). DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Buswell, G. T. (1920). An experimental study of the eye-voice span in reading. Journal of Educational Psychology, 4 (12), 217–227.Google Scholar
(1921). The relationship between eye-perception and voice-response in reading. Journal of Educational Psychology, 12 (4), 217–227. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Carr, J. W., Pescuma, V. N., Furlan, M., Ktori, M., & Crepaldi, D. (2021). Algorithms for the automated correction of vertical drift in eye-tracking data. Behavior Research Methods, 1–24.Google Scholar
Cohen, J. D., Servan-Schreiber, D., & McClelland, J. L. (1992). A parallel distributed processing approach to automaticity. The American journal of psychology, 239–269. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Crepaldi, D., Ferro, M., Marzi, C., Nadalini, A., Pirrelli, V., & Taxitari, L. (2022). Finger movements and eye movements during adults’ silent and oral reading. In R. Levie, A. Bar-On, O. Ashkenazi, E. Dattner, & G. Brandes (Eds.), Developing Language and Literacy: Studies in Honor of Dorit Diskin Ravid (pp. 443–471). Springer International Publishing. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Dell’Orletta, F., Federico, M., Lenci, A., Montemagni, S., & Pirrelli, V. (2007). Maximum Entropy for Italian PoS Tagging. Intelligenza Artificiale, 4 (2).Google Scholar
Dell’Orletta, F., Montemagni, S., & Venturi, G. (2011). READ-IT: Assessing readability of Italian texts with a view to text simplification. In Proceedings of the second workshop on Speech and Language Processing for Assistive Technologies (pp. 73–83).Google Scholar
De Luca, M., Pontillo, M., Primativo, S., Spinelli, D., & Zoccolotti, P. (2013). The eye-voice lead during oral reading in developmental dyslexia. Frontiers in human neuroscience, 7 1, 696. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Drieghe, D., Rayner, K., & Pollatsek, A. (2008). Mislocated fixations can account for parafoveal-on-foveal effects in eye movements during reading. Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 61 (8), 1239–1249. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Federici, S., Montemagni, S., & Pirrelli, V. (1996). Shallow Parsing and Text Chunking: a View on Underspecification in Syntax. Proceedings of ESSLLI’96 Workshop on Robust Parsing, 35–44.Google Scholar
Ferro, M., Cappa, C., Giulivi, S., Marzi, C., Nahli, O., Cardillo, F. A., & Pirrelli, V. (2018). Readlet: Reading for understanding. In Proceedings of 5th IEEE Congress on Information Science & Technology (IEEE CiST’18). Marrakech, Morocco. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Fuchs, L. S., Fuchs, D., Hosp, M. K., & Jenkins, J. R. (2001). Oral reading fluency as an indicator of reading competence: A theoretical, empirical, and historical analysis. Scientific studies of reading, 5 (3), 239–256. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Gathercole, S. E. (2006). Nonword repetition and word learning: The nature of the relationship. Applied psycholinguistics, 27 (4), 513–543. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Grabe, W., & Stoller, F. L. (2019). Teaching and researching reading (3rd ed.). Routledge. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Hughes, B., McClelland, A., & Henare, D. (2014). On the nonsmooth, nonconstant velocity of braille reading and reversals. Scientific Studies of Reading, 18 (2), 94–113. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Huovinen, E., Ylitalo, A.-K., & Puurtinen, M. (2018). Early attraction in temporally controlled sight reading of music. Journal of Eye Movement Research, 11 (2), 1–30. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Hyönä, J., & Bertram, R. (2004). Do frequency characteristics of nonfixated words influence the processing of fixated words during reading? European Journal of Cognitive Psychology, 16 (1–2), 104–127. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Inhoff, A. W., Solomon, M., Radach, R., & Seymour, B. A. (2011). Temporal dynamics of the eyevoice span and eye movement control during oral reading. Journal of Cognitive Psychology, 23 (5), 543–558. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Inhoff, A. W., Starr, M., & Shindler, K. L. (2000). Is the processing of words during eye fixations in reading strictly serial? Perception & Psychophysics, 62 (7), 1474–1484. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Kentner, G., & Vasishth, S. (2016). Prosodic focus marking in silent reading: effects of discourse context and rhythm. Frontiers in Psychology, 7 1, 319. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Kliegl, R., Nuthmann, A., & Engbert, R. (2006). Tracking the mind during reading: The influence of past, present, and future words on fixation durations. Journal of experimental psychology: General, 135 (1), 12. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Kumle, L., Võ, M. L.-H., & Draschkow, D. (2021). Estimating power in (generalized) linear mixed models: An open introduction and tutorial in r. Behavior research methods, 53 (6), 2528–2543. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Laubrock, J., & Kliegl, R. (2015). The eye-voice span during reading aloud. Frontiers in psychology, 6 1, 1432. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Lawson, E. A. (1961). A note on the influence of different orders of approximation to the english language upon eye-voice span. Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 13 (1), 53–55. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Lenci, A., Montemagni, S., & Pirrelli, V. (2003). “Chunk-it”. An Italian Shallow Parser for Robust Syntactic Annotation. Linguistica Computazionale, XVIII–XIX 1, 353–386.Google Scholar
Levin, H., & Cohn, J. A. (1968). Effects of instruction on the eye-voice span. In H. Levin, E. J. Gibson, & J. J. Gibson (Eds.), The analysis of reading skills: A program of basic and applied research. final report (pp. 254–283). Ithaca, New York: Cornell University.Google Scholar
Levin, H., & Turner, E. A. (1968). Sentence structure and the eye-voice span. In H. Levin, E. J. Gibson, & J. J. Gibson (Eds.), The analysis of reading skills: A program of basic and applied research. final report (pp. 196–220). Ithaca, New York: Cornell University.Google Scholar
Lim, Y., Park, J. M., Rhyu, S.-Y., Chung, C. K., Kim, Y., & Yi, S. W. (2019). Eye-hand span is not an indicator of but a strategy for proficient sight-reading in piano performance. Scientific Reports, 9 (1), 1–11. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Lio, G., Fadda, R., Doneddu, G., Duhamel, J.-R., & Sirigu, A. (2019). Digit-tracking as a new tactile interface for visual perception analysis. Nature Communications, 10 (5392), 1–13.Google Scholar
Lyding, V., Stemle, E., Borghetti, C., Brunello, M., Castagnoli, S., Dell’ Orletta, F., … Pirrelli, V. (2014). The Paisa corpus of Italian web texts. In F. Bildhauer & R. Schäfer (Eds.), (p. 36–43). Gothenburg, Sweden: Association for Computational Linguistics.Google Scholar
Maffei, L. (2018). Elogio della parola. Bologna: il Mulino.Google Scholar
Marzi, C., Rodella, A., Nadalini, A., Taxitari, L., & Pirrelli, V. (2020). Does finger-tracking point to child reading strategies? In J. Monti, F. Dell’Orletta, & F. Tamburini (Eds.), Proceedings of 7th Italian Conference on Computational Linguistics (Vol. 27691). Bologna. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Mesmer, H. A. E., & Lake, K. (2010). The role of syllable awareness and syllable-controlled text in the development of finger-point reading. Reading Psychology, 31 (2), 176–201. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Mesmer, H. A. E., & Williams, T. O. (2015). Examining the role of syllable awareness in a model of concept of word: Findings from preschoolers. Reading Research Quarterly, 50 (4), 483–497. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Moors, A. (2016). Automaticity: Componential, causal, and mechanistic explanations. Annual review of psychology, 67 1, 263–287. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Morton, J. (1964a). The effects of context upon speed of reading, eye movements and eye-voice span. Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 16 (4), 340–354. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
(1964b). A model for continuous language behaviour. Language and Speech, 7 (1), 40–70. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Nonaka, T., Ito, K., & Stoffregen, T. A. (2021). Structure of variability in scanning movement predicts braille reading performance in children. Scientific reports, 11 (1), 1–12. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Paap, K. R., & Noel, R. W. (1991). Dual-route models of print to sound: Still a good horse race. Psychological research, 53 (1), 13–24. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Pate, J. K., & Goldwater, S. (2011). Unsupervised syntactic chunking with acoustic cues: computational models for prosodic bootstrapping. In Proceedings of the 2nd Workshop on Cognitive Modeling and Computational Linguistics (pp. 20–29).Google Scholar
Pollatsek, A., & Treiman, R. (2015). The oxford handbook of reading. Oxford University Press. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Povey, D., Ghoshal, A., Boulianne, G., Burget, L., Glembek, O., Goel, N., … Vesely, K. (2011). The Kaldi Speech Recognition Toolkit. In IEEE 2011 Workshop on Automatic Speech Recognition and Understanding. IEEE Signal Processing Society. (IEEE Catalog No.: CFP11SRW-USB)Google Scholar
Protopapas, A., Altani, A., & Georgiou, G. K. (2013). Development of serial processing in reading and rapid naming. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 116 (4), 914–929. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
R Core Team. (2023). R: A language and environment for statistical computing. [URL]. Vienna, Austria: R Foundation for Statistical Computing.
Reichle, E. D. (2021). Computational models of reading: A handbook. Oxford University Press. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Sakoe, H., & Chiba, S. (1978). Dynamic programming algorithm optimization for spoken word recognition. IEEE Transactions on Acoustics, Speech, and Signal Processing, 26 (1), 43–49. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Schilling, S. G., Carlisle, J. F., Scott, S. E., & Zeng, J. (2007). Are fluency measures accurate predictors of reading achievement? The Elementary School Journal, 107 (5), 429–448. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Shmyrev, N. V., & Vosk Core Team. (2020). Vosk Speech Recognition Toolkit: Offline speech recognition API for Android, iOS, Raspberry Pi and servers with Python, Java, C# and Node. [URL]. GitHub repository.
Silva, S., Reis, A., Casaca, L., Petersson, K. M., & Faísca, L. (2016). When the Eyes no longer lead: Familiarity and Length Effects on Eye-Voice Span. Frontiers in Psychology, 7 1. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Taxitari, L., Cappa, C., Ferro, M., Marzi, C., Nadalini, A., & Pirrelli, V. (2021). Using mobile technology for reading assessment. In Proceedings of 6th IEEE Congress on Information Science & Technology (IEEE CiST’20). Agadir, Morocco.Google Scholar
Uhry, J. K. (1999). Invented spelling in kindergarten: The relationship with finger-point reading. Reading and Writing, 11 1, 441–464. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
(2002). Finger-point reading in kindergarten: The role of phonemic awareness, one-to-one correspondence, and rapid serial naming. Scientific Studies of Reading, 6 (4), 319–342. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Vihman, M. M. (2022). The developmental origins of phonological memory. Psychological review, 129 (6), 1495–1508. DOI logoGoogle Scholar