Article published in:
The Mental Lexicon
Vol. 12:1 (2017) ► pp. 2150

Full-text

L2 processing of Arabic derivational morphology
References

[ p. 41 ]References

Babcock, L., Stowe, J. C., Maloof, C. J., Brovetto, C., & Ullman, M. T.
(2012) The storage and composition of inflected forms in adult-learned second language: A study of the influence of length of residence, age of arrival, sex, and other factors. Bilingualism: Language and Cognition, 15(4), 820–840. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Basnight-Brown, D. M., Chen, L., Hua, S., Kostic, A., & Feldman, L. B.
(2007) Monolingual and bilingual recognition of regular and irregular English verbs: Sensitivity to form similarity varies with first language experience. Journal of Memory and Language, 57(1), 65–80. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Bat-El, O.
(1994) Stem modification and cluster transfer in Modern Hebrew. Natural Language & Linguistic Theory, 12(4), 571–596. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
(2003) Semitic verb structure within a universal perspective. Language Acquisition and Language Disorders, 28, 29–60. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Bentin, S., & Frost, R.
(1994) Morphologocal factors in visual word identification in Hebrew. In L. Feldman (Ed.) Morphological aspects of language processing. (pp. 271–292). N.J., USA: Erlbaum.Google Scholar
Boudelaa, S.
(2014) Is the Arabic mental lexicon morpheme-based or stem-based? Implications for spoken and written word recognition. In Handbook of Arabic Literacy (pp. 31–54). Springer Netherlands. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Boudelaa, S., & Marslen-Wilson, W. D.
(2000) Non-concatenative morphemes in language processing: Evidence from Modern Standard Arabic. In ISCA Tutorial and Research Workshop (ITRW) on Spoken Word Access Processes, 23–26.Google Scholar
Boudelaa, S. & Marslen-Wilson, W. D.
(2001) Morphological units in the Arabic mental lexicon, Cognition, 81, 65–92. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
(2004) Allomorphic variation in Arabic: implications for lexical processing and representation. Brain and Language, 90, 106–116. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
(2005) Discontinuous morphology in time: Incremental masked priming in Arabic. Language and Cognitive Processes, 20, 207–260. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
(2011) Productivity and priming: Morphemic decomposition in Arabic. Language and Cognitive Processes, 26, 624–652. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
(2013) Morphological structure in the Arabic mental lexicon: Parallels between standard and dialectal Arabic. Language and Cognitive Processes. 28(10), 1453–1473. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Clahsen, H., Balkhair, L., Schutter, J-S., & Cunnings, I.
(2013) The time course of morphological processing in a second language. Second Language Research, 29(1), 7–31. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Clahsen, H., Felser, C., Neubauer, K., Sato M., & Silva R.
(2010) Morphological structure in native and non-native language processing. Language Learning, 60, 21–43. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Cook, S. V., & Gor, K.
(2015) Lexical access in L2: Representational deficit or processing constraint? The Mental Lexicon, 10(2), 247–270. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Coughlin, C. E., & Tremblay, A. (2015) Morphological decomposition in native and non-native French speakers. Bilingualism: Language and Cognition, 18(03), 524–542. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Deutsch, A., & Frost, R.
(2003) Lexical organization and lexical access in a non- concatenated morphology: Mapping the mental lexicon. In Y. Shimron (Ed.) Language processing and acquisition in Languages of Semitic, Root Based Morphology. Amsterdam/ Philadelphia: John Benjamins. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Diependaele, K., Duñabeitia, J. A., Morris, J., & Keuleers, E.
(2011) Fast Morphological Effects in First and Second Language Word Recognition. Journal of Memory and Language, 64, 344–358. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Diependaele, K., Sandra, D. & Grainger, J.
(2005) Masked cross-modal priming: Unravelling morpho-orthographic and morpho-semantic influences in early word recognition. Language and Cognitive Processes, 20(1–2), 75–114. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Feldman, L. B., Barac-Cikoja, D. & Kostić, A.
(2002) Semantic aspects of morphological processing: Transparency effects in Serbian. Memory & Cognition, 30(4), 629–636. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Feldman, L. B., Frost, R., & Pnini, T.
(1995) Decomposing words into their constituent morphemes: evidence from English and Hebrew. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 21, 947–960.Google Scholar
Feldman, L. B., Kostić, A., Basnight-Brown, D. M., Filipović Đurđević, D., Pastizzo, M. J.
(2010) Morphological facilitation for regular and irregular verb formations in native and non-native speakers: Little evidence for two distinct mechanisms. Bilingualism: Language and Cognition, 13, 119–135. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Feldman, L. B., O’Connor, P. A., Moscoso del Prado Martín, F.
(2009) Early morphological processing is morphosyntactic and not simply morpho-orthographic: A violation of form-then-meaning accounts of word recognition. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 16(4), 684–691. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Feldman, L. B., & Soltano, E. G.
(1999) Morphological priming: The role of prime duration, semantic transparency, and affix position. Brain & Language, 68, 33–39. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Foote, R.
(2015) The storage and processing of morphologically complex words in L2 Spanish. Studies in Second Language Acquisition, pp. 1–33. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Forster, K. I. & Forster, J. C.
(2003) DMDX: A windows display program with millisecond accuracy. Behavior Research Methods, Instruments, & Computers, 35(1), 116–124. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Frost, R., Deutsch, A. & Forster, K. I.
(2000) Decomposing Morphologically Complex Words in a Nonlinear Morphology. Journal of Experimental Psychology Learning Memory, & Cognition, 26, 751–765. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Frost, R., Deutsch, A., Gilboa, O., Tannenbaum, M. & Marslen-Wilson, W.
(2000) Morphological Priming: Dissociation of phonological, semantic, and morphological factors. Memory & Cognition, 28(6), 1277–1288. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Gor, K.
(2010) Beyond the obvious: Do second language learners process inflectional morphology? Language Learning, 60(1), 1–20. Introduction to the thematic issue. Guest editor K. Gor. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Gor, K., & Cook, S.
(2010) Non-native processing of verbal morphology: In search of regularity. Language Learning, 60(1), 88–126. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Gor, K., & Jackson, S. (2013) Morphological decomposition and lexical access in a native and second language: A nesting doll effect. Language and Cognitive Processes, 28(7), 1065–1091. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Hamburger, M., & Slowiaczek, L. M.
(1996) Phonological priming reflects lexical competition. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 3(4), 520–525. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Holes, C.
(1995) Modern Arabic: structures, functions, and varieties. London: Longman.Google Scholar
Jacob, G., Fleischhauer, E. & Clahsen, H.
(2013) Allomorphy and afixation in morphological processing: A cross-modal priming study with late bilinguals. Bilingualism: Language and Cognition, 16, 924–933. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Jiang, N.
(2000) Lexical representation and development in a second language. Applied Linguistics, 21(1), 47–77. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Kieffer, M. J. & Lesaux, N. K.
(2008) The role of derivational morphology in the reading comprehension of Spanish-speaking English language learners. Reading and Writing, 21, 783–804. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Kirkici, B. & Clahsen, H.
(2013) Inflection and derivation in native and non-native language processing: Masked priming experiments in Turkish. Bilingualism: Language and Cognition, 16(4), 776–791. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Kossmann, M.
(2013) The Arabic influence on northern Berber. Leiden, The Netherlands: Brill. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Kroll, J. F. & Stewart, E.
(1994) Category interference in translation and picture naming: Evidence for asymmetric connections between bilingual memory representations. Journal of Memory and Language, 33(2), 149–174. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Longtin, C., Segui, J. & Hallé, P. A.
(2003) Morphological priming without morphological relationship. Language and Cognitive Processes, 18(3), 313–334. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Marslen-Wilson, W., Tyler, L., Waksler, R., & Older, L.
(1994) Morphology and meaning in the English mental lexicon. Psychological Review, 101, 3–33. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
McCarthy, J. J.
(1979) Formal problems in Semitic morphology and phonology. Dissertation MIT, Cambridge, MA.Google Scholar
(1981) A prosodic theory of nonconcatenative morphology. Linguistic inquiry, 12(3), 373–418.Google Scholar
McCarthy, J., & Prince, A.
(1990) Prosodic morphology and templatic morphology. In Perspectives on Arabic linguistics II: papers from the second annual symposium on Arabic linguistics (pp. 1–54).Google Scholar
McDonald, J. L.
(2006) Beyond the critical period: Processing-based explanations for poor grammaticality judgment performance by late second language learners. Journal of Memory and Language, 55, 381–401. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Neubauer, K. & Clahsen, H.
(2009) Decomposition of inflected words in a second language: An experimental study of German participles. Studies in Second Language Acquisition, 31, 403–35. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Perea, M., & Lupker, S. J.
(2003) Transposed-letter confusability effects in masked form priming. In S. Kinoshita and S. J. Lupker (Eds.), Masked priming: State of the art (pp. 97–120). Hove, UK: Psychology Press.Google Scholar
Perea, M., Abu Mallouh, R., & Carreiras, M.
(2010) The search of an input coding scheme: Transposed-letter priming in Arabic. Psychonomic Bulletin and Review, 17, 375–380. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Portin, M., Lehtonen, M., & Laine, M. (2007) Processing of inflected nouns in late bilinguals. Applied Psycholinguistics, 28 (1), 135–156. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Portin, M., Lehtonen, M., Harrer, G., Wande, E., Niemi, J., & Laine, M.
(2008) L1 effects on the processing of inflected nouns in L2. Acta Psychologica, 128 (3), 452–465. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Prunet, J., Beland, R. & Idrissi, A.
(2000) The Mental Representation of Semitic Words. Linguistic Inquiry, 31, 609–648. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Rastle, K., Davis, M. H., Marslen-Wilson, W. D., & Tyler, L. K.
(2000) Morphological and semantic effects in visual word recognition: A time course study. Language and cognitive processes. 15(4/5), 507–537. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Rastle, K., Lavric, A., Elchlepp, H., & Crepaldi, D.
(2015) Processing differences across regular and irregular inflections revealed through ERPs. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 41(3), 747–760.Google Scholar
Ratcliffe, R.
(1997) Prosodic templates in a word-based morphological analysis of Arabic. In Eid, M. and R. R. Ratcliffe (eds.) Perspectives on Arabic Linguistics X, Papers from the tenth annual symposium on Arabic linguistics, pp. 147–172. John Benjamins, Amsterdam. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Ratcliffe, R. R.
(2004) Sonority-based parsing at the margins of Arabic morphology: In response to Prunet, Béland and Idrissi (2000) and Davis and Zawaydeh (1999, 2001). Al-'Arabiyya, 37, 7395.Google Scholar
Silva, R., & Clahsen, H.
(2008) Morphologically complex words in L1 and L2 processing: Evidence from masked priming experiments in English. Bilingualism: Language and Cognition, 11, 245–260. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Slowiaczek, L. M., & Hamburger, M.
(1992) Prelexical facilitation and lexical interference in auditory word recognition. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 18(6), 1239–1250.Google Scholar
Ussishkin, A.
(2000) The emergence of fixed prosody. Ph.D. thesis, University of California at Santa Cruz.Google Scholar
(2003) Templatic effects as fixed prosody: The verbal system in Semitic. Amsterdam studies in the theory and history of linguistic science series, 4, pp. 511–530.Google Scholar
(2005) A Fixed Prosodic Theory of Nonconcatenative Templatic Morphology. Natural Language & Linguistic Theory, 23(1), pp. 169–218. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Ussishkin, A., Dawson, C. R., Wedel, A., & Schluter, K.
(2015) Auditory masked priming in Maltese spoken word recognition. Language, Cognition and Neuroscience, 30(9), 1096–1115. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Velan, H., & Frost, R.
(2009) Letter-transposition effects are not universal: The impact of transposing letters in Hebrew. Journal of Memory & Language, 61, 285–302. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Versteegh, K.
(1997) The Arabic Language. New York: Columbia University Press.Google Scholar
Voga, М., Anastassiadis-Symeonidis, А., & Giraudo, H.
(2014) Does morphology play a role in L2 processing? Two masked priming experiments with Greek speakers of ESL. Lingvisticae Investigationes, 37, 338–352. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Cited by

Cited by 7 other publications

Al-Lahham, Yaser A.
2021. Index Term Selection Heuristics for Arabic Text Retrieval. Arabian Journal for Science and Engineering 46:4  pp. 3345 ff. Crossref logo
Asli-Badarneh, Abeer & Mark Leikin
2019. Morphological ability among monolingual and bilingual speakers in early childhood: The case of two Semitic languages. International Journal of Bilingualism 23:5  pp. 1087 ff. Crossref logo
FARHY, YAEL, JOÃO VERÍSSIMO & HARALD CLAHSEN
2018. Do late bilinguals access pure morphology during word recognition? A masked-priming study on Hebrew as a second language. Bilingualism: Language and Cognition 21:5  pp. 945 ff. Crossref logo
Foote, Rebecca, Mousa Qasem & Emma Trentman
2020. Morphological Decomposition in L2 Arabic: A Masked Priming Study. Journal of Psycholinguistic Research 49:2  pp. 291 ff. Crossref logo
Kahraman, Hasibe & Bilal Kırkıcı
2021. Letter transpositions and morphemic boundaries in the second language processing of derived words: An exploratory study of individual differences. Applied Psycholinguistics 42:2  pp. 417 ff. Crossref logo
Song, Yoonsang, Youngah Do, Arthur L. Thompson, Eileen R. Waegemaekers & Jongbong Lee
2020. SECOND LANGUAGE USERS EXHIBIT SHALLOW MORPHOLOGICAL PROCESSING. Studies in Second Language Acquisition 42:5  pp. 1121 ff. Crossref logo
Sánchez, Liliana & Elisabeth Mayer
2018. Typological Differences in Morphological Patterns, Gender Features, and Thematic Structure in the L2 Acquisition of Ashaninka Spanish. Languages 3:2  pp. 21 ff. Crossref logo

This list is based on CrossRef data as of 30 august 2021. Please note that it may not be complete. Sources presented here have been supplied by the respective publishers. Any errors therein should be reported to them.